正在加载图片...
REVIEWS Table 1|Different ways of classifying behaviour egory of behaviour in general h ther pe Personaity traits (extraversion,neuroticism) swith Socal emotions (u embarrassment.pride.jealousy data that ane a ilabl ent r as man motional response Basic emoions (happiness.tear,anger,disgust,sadness) Motivational state (reward,punishment) finding that we accumulate be under a ing ks exn n the m ent ns of mnnate and acqured f are th cultu more cos e and e be ood b art by fro nd to what suit be recly shape 5d ng.Y.Card, n11255-6158 30. 00 D.&E s,R Th en,E.Ral, 41513 38 onu a2 lonsinapatert with bteral 199门9 E 1656116 30. 44 T.ela.Ar S.E.A 41,305 A.Gros Gabnell.D.E 176 MARCH 2003 VOLUME 176 | MARCH 2003 | VOLUME 4 www.nature.com/reviews/neuro REVIEWS re-describe events from several points of view — an ability that might fuel the emergence of science, art and culture in general. Just as we can think about other peo￾ple, we can step outside ourselves and think about our￾selves, have conversations with ourselves, and imagine things happening to ourselves in the future. The data that are available at present raise as many questions as they provide answers. How can the diverse findings that we accumulate be situated under a single functional framework? Specifically, how can causal net￾works explain the many correlations between brain and behaviour that we are discovering? What are the relative contributions of innate and acquired factors, culture and individual differences to social cognition? To what extent do these factors contribute to psychopathology? Can large-scale social behaviour, as studied by political science and economics, be understood by studying social cognition in individual subjects? Finally, what power will insights from cognitive neuroscience give us to influence our social behaviour, and hence society? And to what extent would such pursuit be morally defensible? How we approach these questions will largely shape social brain science in the coming decades. reward and punishment148. We might need to invent a new set of terms that can translate between the different ways of describing social behaviour, and that correspond more closely to the neural processes that underlie them. It might be that certain social cognitive skills — notably the ability to represent other people’s minds — distinguish humans and perhaps apes from all other animals. If we understand other people in part by simu￾lating processes within ourselves, the converse is also true: we understand ourselves in part by observing other people and their reactions to us. Our ability to think about other people might be an aspect of our ability to Table 1 | Different ways of classifying behaviour Category of behaviour Example Social disposition Personality traits (extraversion, neuroticism) Strategic Deception, reconciliation Ecological Attachment, aggression Moral Social emotions (guilt, embarrassment, pride, jealousy) Emotional response Basic emotions (happiness, fear, anger, disgust, sadness) Reinforcement Motivational state (reward, punishment) 1. Adolphs, R. (ed.) Special issue on cognitive neuroscience of social behavior. Neuropsychologia 41, 117 (2003). 2. Harmon-Jones, E. & Devine, T. (eds) Special issue on social neuroscience. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. (in the press). 3. Cacioppo, J. T. et al. (eds) Foundations in Social Neuroscience (MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, 2001). 4. Heatherton, T. F. & Macrae, C. N. Social Cognitive Neuroscience: A Reader (Blackwell, Cambridge, Massachusetts, 2003). 5. Trivers, R. The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Q. Rev. Biol. 46, 35–57 (1971). 6. Fehr, E. & Gaechter, S. Altruistic punishment in humans. Nature 415, 137–140 (2002). 7. Edwards, K. The face of time: temporal cues in facial expressions of emotion. Psychol. Sci. 9, 270–277 (1998). 8. Ambady, N. & Rosenthal, R. Thin slices of expressive behavior as predictors of interpersonal consequences: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 111, 256–274 (1992). 9. McCarthy, G. in The New Cognitive Neurosciences (ed. Gazzaniga, M. S.) 393–410 (MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, 1999). 10. Haxby, J. V., Hoffman, E. A. & Gobbini, M. I. The distributed human neural system for face perception. Trends Cogn. Sci. 4, 223–233 (2000). 11. Wicker, B., Michel, F., Henaff, A. & Decety, J. Brain regions involved in the perception of gaze: a PET study. Neuroimage 8, 221–227 (1998). 12. Hoffman, E. A. & Haxby, J. V. Distinct representations of eye gaze and identity in the distributed human neural system for face perception. Nature Neurosci. 3, 80–84 (2000). This paper shows that the superior temporal and fusiform cortices are differentially activated by eye gaze and static face stimuli, respectively. 13. Puce, A., Allison, T., Bentin, S., Gore, J. C. & McCarthy, G. Temporal cortex activation in humans viewing eye and mouth movements. J. Neurosci. 18, 2188–2199 (1998). 14. Vaina, L. M., Solomon, J., Chowdhury, S., Sinha, P. & Belliveau, J. W. Functional neuroanatomy of biological motion perception in humans. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 98, 11656–11661 (2001). 15. Iacoboni, M. et al. Reafferent copies of imitated actions in the right superior temporal cortex. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 98, 13995–13999 (2001). 16. Vuilleumier, P., Armony, J. L., Driver, J. & Dolan, R. J. Effects of attention and emotion on face processing in the human brain. An event-related fMRI study. Neuron 30, 829–841 (2001). 17. Pelphrey, K. A., Singerman, J. D., Allison, T. & McCarthy, G. Brain activation evoked by the perception of gaze shifts: the influence of timing and context. Neuropsychologia 41, 156–170 (2003). 18. Wicker, B., Perrett, D. I., Baron-Cohen, S. & Decety, J. Being the target of another’s emotion: a PET study. Neuropsychologia 41, 139–146 (2003). 19. Mouchetant-Rostaing, Y., Giard, M.-H., Bentin, S., Aguera, P.-E. & Pernier, J. Neurophysiological correlates of face gender processing in humans. Eur. J. Neurosci. 12, 303–310 (2000). 20. Pizzagalli, D., Regard, M. & Lehmann, D. Rapid emotional face processing in the human right and left brain hemispheres: an ERP study. Neuroreport 10, 2691–2698 (1999). 21. Halgren, E., Raij, T., Marinkovic, K., Jousmaki, V. & Hari, R. Cognitive response profile of the human fusiform face area as determined by MEG. Cereb. Cortex 10, 69–81 (2000). 22. Smith, N. K., Cacioppo, J. T., Larsen, J. T. & Chartrand, T. L. May I have your attention, please: electrocortical responses to positive and negative stimuli. Neuropsychologia 41, 171–183 (2003). 23. Allison, T. et al. Face recognition in human extrastriate cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 71, 821–825 (1994). 24. Liu, J., Harris, A. & Kanwisher, N. Stages of processing in face perception: an MEG study. Nature Neurosci. 5, 910–916 (2002). Demonstrates that superordinate categorization of a face as distinct from other objects at 100 ms is followed by subordinate categorization of individual people’s faces around 170 ms. 25. Sugase, Y., Yamane, S., Ueno, S. & Kawano, K. Global and fine information coded by single neurons in the temporal visual cortex. Nature 400, 869–872 (1999). 26. Heider, F. & Simmel, M. An experimental study of apparent behavior. Am. J. Psychol. 57, 243–259 (1944). A classic study showing that people make social attributions to moving geometric shapes. 27. Michotte, A. La Perception de la Causalite (Institut Superieur de Philosophie, Louvain, France, 1946). 28. Scholl, B. J. & Tremoulet, P. D. Perceptual causality and animacy. Trends Cogn. Sci. 4, 299–308 (2000). 29. Dittrich, W. H. & Lea, S. E. G. Visual perception of intentional motion. Perception 23, 253–268 (1994). 30. Johansson, G. Visual perception of biological motion and a model of its analysis. Percept. Psychophys. 14, 202–211 (1973). Classic study showing that we perceive people’s bodies from point-light displays. 31. Cutting, J. E. & Kozlowski, L. T. Recognizing friends by their walk: gait perception without familiarity cues. Bull. Psychon. Soc. 9, 353–356 (1977). 32. Kozlowski, L. T. & Cutting, J. E. Recognizing the sex of a walker from a dynamic point-light display. Percept. Psychophys. 21, 575–580 (1977). 33. Dittrich, W. H., Troscianko, T., Lea, S. E. & Morgan, D. Perception of emotion from point-light displays represented in dance. Perception 25, 727–738 (1996). 34. Runeson, S. & Frykholm, G. Kinematic specification of dynamics as an informational basis for person-and-action perception: expectation, gender recognition, and deceptive intention. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 112, 585–615 (1983). 35. Grezes, J. et al. Does perception of biological motion rely on specific brain regions? Neuroimage 13, 775–785 (2001). 36. Bonda, E., Petrides, M., Ostry, D. & Evans, A. Specific involvement of human parietal systems and the amygdala in the perception of biological motion. J. Neurosci. 16, 3737–3744 (1996). 37. Grossman, E. & Blake, R. Brain areas active during visual perception of biological stimuli. Neuron 35, 1167–1175 (2002). 38. Castelli, F., Happe, F., Frith, U. & Frith, C. Movement and mind: a functional imaging study of perceptions and interpretation of complex intentional movement patterns. Neuroimage 12, 314–325 (2000). 39. Blakemore, S.-J. et al. How the brain perceives causality: an event-related fMRI study. Neuroreport 12, 3741–3746 (2001). 40. Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., Damasio, H. & Damasio, A. Impaired recognition of emotion in facial expressions following bilateral damage to the human amygdala. Nature 372, 669–672 (1994). The first study showing impaired recognition of fear from facial expressions in a patient with bilateral amygdala damage. 41. Calder, A. J. et al. Facial emotion recognition after bilateral amygdala damage: differentially severe impairment of fear. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 13, 699–745 (1996). 42. Morris, J. S. et al. A differential neural response in the human amygdala to fearful and happy facial expressions. Nature 383, 812–815 (1996). The first functional imaging study showing amygdala activation to facial expressions of fear. 43. Hamann, S. B., Ely, T. D., Hoffman, J. M. & Kilts, C. D. Ecstasy and agony: activation of the human amygdala in positive and negative emotion. Psycholog. Sci. 13, 135–141 (2002). 44. Yang, T. T. et al. Amygdalar activation associated with positive and negative facial expressions. Neuroreport 13, 1737–1741 (2002). 45. Anderson, A. K. & Phelps, E. A. Lesions of the human amygdala impair enhanced perception of emotionally salient events. Nature 411, 305–309 (2001). Shows that the ability of emotional stimuli to override the attentional blink depends on the amygdala. 46. Ochsner, K., Bunge, S. A., Gross, J. J. & Gabrieli, J. D. E. Rethinking feelings: an fMRI study of the cognitive
<<向上翻页向下翻页>>
©2008-现在 cucdc.com 高等教育资讯网 版权所有