Review setstdntr Imaging the developing brain:what have we learned about cognitive development? B.J.Casey',Nim Tottenham1,Conor Liston'and Sarah Durston1.2 Sackler nstitute for Developmental Psychobiology Weill Medical orell University,3 York Avenue,Box 140. dolescn Pychatry University Medical Crect,Hidr 100.xech The Netherlands function.changes in the yolume of a structure or of activity s measured by MR methods lack the resolution ross the Iite spa ro tech characterize the mechanism of change definitive these change s safely in the human in vivo.We review dynamic essive change the imaging lite ve e Box ed nendent cha nd and change in the atomy across childhood the methodolog gies provide ngs s ggest th nne-tune d in conjun basic functic Follation areas mo ce mature fi onteoiofbehx s but also repeatec canning of th of g eral neuroanatomical changes with both learning and evelophishtMR-bas development. he neural and cognitive development.This review what we have learned about the bi limitations of the methods,and the relevance of these ngs,providing ing and ons fo development.Claims of causalitybetween coincidenta hes gin g methods de with the opportu underlying human development see Box D).These methods hay e advanced the field developmental of changes with tural architecture and functional the of childh general,the sequence in which the cortex matures escognitive milestones in human Casey BJ.(bie200 and con .dot10.101 6Ltica.200s.01.01
Imaging the developing brain: what have we learned about cognitive development? B.J. Casey1 , Nim Tottenham1 , Conor Liston1 and Sarah Durston1,2 1 Sackler Institute for Developmental Psychobiology, Weill Medical College of Cornell University, 1300 York Avenue, Box 140, New York, NY 10021, USA 2 Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, University Medical Center Utrecht, Heidelberglaan 100, 3584 CX Utrecht, The Netherlands The human brain undergoes significant changes in both its structural architecture and functional organization across the life span. Advances in neuroimaging techniques over the past decade have allowed us to track these changes safely in the human in vivo. We review the imaging literature on the neurobiology of cognitive development, focusing specifically on cognitive taskdependent changes observed in brain physiology and anatomy across childhood and adolescence. The findings suggest that cortical function becomes fine-tuned with development. Brain regions associated with more basic functions such as sensory and motor processes mature first, followed by association areas involved in top-down control of behavior. Introduction The year 2005 marks a decade since the first application of fMRI to developmental questions [1]. It is well established that brain development and cognitive maturation occur concurrently during childhood and adolescence [2–4], but much less is known about the direct relationship between neural and cognitive development. This review highlights what we have learned about the biological substrates of cognitive development over the past decade, advances and limitations of the methods, and the relevance of these studies to understanding and developing interventions for individuals with atypical development. Emphasis is placed on studies that inform how cognitive development and learning map onto changes in brain anatomy, connectivity and physiology across childhood and adolescence. Contemporary non-invasive neuroimaging methods have provided developmental scientists with the opportunity to track safely cognitive and neural processes underlying human development (see Box 1). These methods have advanced the field of developmental neuroscience by providing evidence of changes in structural architecture and functional organization in the developing brain in vivo. However, these measures provide only an indirect measure of brain structure and function. Changes in the volume of a structure or amount of activity as measured by MR methods lack the resolution to characterize the mechanism of change definitively. Histological evidence suggests that brain development is a dynamic process of regressive and progressive changes (see Box 2). As such, MRI-based cortical changes observed with development may be a combination of myelination, dendritic pruning and changes in the vascular, neuronal and glial density. Nonetheless, the methodologies provide information about regional development that, in conjunction with histological studies, could tease these processes apart. Furthermore, because these tools permit not only scanning of children but also repeated scanning of the same individual over time, they can provide measurement of general neuroanatomical changes with both learning and development. This review highlights MR-based measures of change in neuroanatomy, cortical connectivity and cortical function with learning and development. Neuroanatomical development of the human brain Several structural imaging studies have mapped the neuroanatomical course of human brain development [5]. The findings parallel many of the post-mortem histological findings, providing validity for in vivo imaging measures, and also parallel behavioral and cognitive development. Claims of causality between coincidental changes in brain and behavioral development is a common trap into which one could fall by simply assuming linear changes across systems and direct associations between these changes. Nonetheless, the synchrony in time course of the changes reported in MRI-based anatomical studies and cognitive development have driven more direct tests of these associations in functional imaging studies. For this reason, MRI-morphometric studies of change in structural architecture are highlighted. The most compelling reports of structural changes with development over the period of childhood and adolescence have come from recent longitudinal MRI studies [6,7]. In general, the sequence in which the cortex matures parallels cognitive milestones in human development [6–9]. First, regions subserving primary functions, such as motor and sensory systems, mature earliest, with Corresponding author: Casey, B.J. (bjc2002@med.cornell.edu). Review TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.9 No.3 March 2005 www.sciencedirect.com 1364-6613/$ - see front matter Q 2005 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.01.011
Review TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.9 No.3 March 2005 105 Box 1.Magnetic resonance methods have introduced nw set of including is for st c that can d ct cha ed by ind non-invasive assumed to reflect”ch (c) mporncriceswth sensorimotor processes and modulate basic attention and and spatia Ru3earoeseeentoathe1ast6n1.specic lateral temporal cortices.which integrate primary loss occurred earliest in the primary sensorimotor areas Box 2.Structural architecture of the developing brain The human brain ami changes in both migration of cem ing pro well into 90%ot It size by and white anges,in subcomponents. Developmental course of human brain development Experie Myelination 8 0246810122468101214161820
temporal and parietal association cortices associated with basic language skills and spatial attention maturing next. Higher-order association areas, such as the prefrontal and lateral temporal cortices, which integrate primary sensorimotor processes and modulate basic attention and language processes, seem to mature last [6,7]. Specifically, MRI-based measures showed that cortical gray matter loss occurred earliest in the primary sensorimotor areas Box 1. Magnetic resonance methods Magnetic resonance technologies have introduced a new set of tools for capturing features of brain development in living, developing humans. The three most common MR methods used in the study of human development and learning are illustrated in Figure I. These imaging methods became especially important to cognitive and developmental scientists when the functional capabilities were discovered. The functional methodology measures changes in blood oxygenation in the brain that are assumed to reflect changes in neural activity [55,56], and eliminates the need for exogenous contrast agents, including radioactive isotopes [57,58]. DTI is a relatively new MR technique that can detect changes in white matter microstructure based on properties of diffusion [59,60]. Diffusion of water in white matter tracts is affected by myelin and the orientation and regularity of fibers and provides an index of brain connectivity. Because all three of these MR techniques are non-invasive, development and learning can be tracked more precisely within individuals with repeat scans over short or long intervals of time (days to years). Figure I. (a) Structural MRI produces structural images of the brain that are useful for anatomical and morphometric studies; (b) DTI measures connectivity of fiber tracts between anatomical structures; and (c) functional MRI measures patterns of brain activity within those structures. Box 2. Structural architecture of the developing brain The human brain undergoes dramatic changes in both its structural architecture and functional organization that reflect a dynamic interplay of simultaneously occurring progressive and regressive events. Although the total brain size is about 90% of adult size by age 6 years, the brain continues to undergo dynamic changes throughout adolescence and well into young adulthood [61]. Figure I illustrates some of these developmental changes, including proliferation and migration of cells mostly during fetal development [62,63], regional changes in synaptic density during postnatal development [11,12,64], and protracted development of myelination well into adulthood [65]. Current non-invasive neuroimaging methods do not have the resolution to delineate which of these processes underlies observed developmental changes beyond gray and white matter subcomponents. TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences –8 –6 –4 –2 0 6 10 12 14 4 8 Developmental course of human brain development Experience-dependent synapse formation and dendritic arborization Synaptogenesis and synaptic pruning Sensorimotor cortex Parietal and temporal association cortex Prefrontal cortex Myelination Conception Birth Months Months Years 2 2 6 10 12 16 18 20 4 8 Neurulation Cell proliferation and migration Figure I. See text for details. Adapted with permission from Ref. [66]. Review TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.9 No.3 March 2005 105 www.sciencedirect.com
106 Review TRENDS in Cognitive Science Vol9 No.3 March 2005 and latest in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex 6.11. information on directionality and regularity of mvelinated These findings are consistent wi non-h and humt e pre imotor cortex in Cr 88-5 indirect meas ures of white matter suggest res norm un ma 10 00 cortex and pres n oncluding that gray matter loss during this per t of working men y was correlated po a sculpt ng process of th bra ely with prefrontal-parieta 124 ent 39,421 t DTI this period of developmentas well 1,13,51.One frontostriatal the report sn subcortical and accrate) performance on a GoNo Th ctional 19 and longitudinal studies 6]sho nt st identified in an fMRI study usin the same task elopment n port ons of the ganglia to 42. Similarly, combined DTI and alytica dual ce ofs this in white of brain correlated with development,but specificity of the invo hite me ement of part r roung adulthood [6,13,14,17,20 evide ed by nitive ability 241Th e dissociations are important ncre density hes cha when distinguishing bet odend l ,both regre Fun tional organization of the develop an brain pr th previo e e nges in bral beingne-tuned with overabun tal volume ing of ele ant conne nt corte How do the ese structural changes rel to cognitive nition is characte hanges: nges cortica developmen ze by the ability to filter and suppress irrelevan prefro lobe stru on and ity to filte and suppres 2Rrprae MRI-b of appropriat es co prefrontal volume asures of cogniti interference competing sources lessening with Many parad ms use dy 21.22).Together th studies su t that,perhaps not and Go/No o-go tasks 33351.Collectively.im n brain dev lopment are task as probe chil tasks than do pment of h man brain connectivity ults sed on ectional [36]and long tudina ctionsa ng revie ned with the 371 reg elimination of an overabundan stre ngthening of of syne es and the focal or fine-tuned,whereas regior elated rele evelopm ase MR s diffusion tensor im TD,P vide a potential tool activity ohser a variety of or examin ing the role of co connectivity in the pment within ides DTI Anding nted previously
and latest in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex [6,10]. These findings are consistent with non-human and human primate postmortem studies showing that the prefrontal cortex matures at a more protracted rate than sensorimotor cortex in synaptic density [11,12]. Cross-sectional studies of normative brain maturation during childhood and adolescence have shown somewhat similar patterns concluding that gray matter loss during this period reflects a sculpting process of the immature brain into the fully functioning mature one [10,13–16]. As such, the pattern of development observed is suggested to reflect the ongoing neuronal regressive events, such as pruning and the elimination of connections. Developmental changes in subcortical regions occur over this period of development as well [10,13,15–19]. One of the more reliable patterns reported is in subcortical regions to which association cortex projects. For example, both cross-sectional [19] and longitudinal studies [6] show patterns of development in portions of the basal ganglia to which the prefrontal cortex directly projects. Again, this pattern could reflect the gradual elimination of connections, with strengthening of others. What is the corresponding pattern of change in white matter volume over this period? In contrast to an inverted U-shaped pattern of development in gray matter with age, white matter volume increases in a roughly linear pattern, increasing throughout development until approximately young adulthood [6,13,14,17,20], as evidenced by increases in volume and density [17]. These changes presumably reflect ongoing myelination of axons by oligodendrocytes enhancing neuronal conduction and communication. Therefore, both regressive and progressive processes are occurring in parallel, which could enhance neural and cognitive processes. Connections are being fine-tuned with the elimination of an overabundance of synapses and strengthening of relevant connections with development and experience. How do these structural changes relate to cognitive changes? Developmental changes in cortical development have been found to correlate with behavioral performance measures. Sowell and colleagues [4] showed an association between prefrontal lobe structural maturation and memory function using neuropsychological measures. Similar associations have been reported between MRI-based prefrontal volume and specific measures of cognitive control (i.e. the ability to override an inappropriate response in favor of another) in cross-sectional studies [21,22]. Together these studies suggest that, perhaps not surprisingly, functional changes in brain development are reflected in structural changes. Development of human brain connectivity The MRI-based morphometry studies reviewed suggest that cortical connections are being fine-tuned with the elimination of an overabundance of synapses and the strengthening of relevant connections with development and experience. Recent advances in MR technology, such as diffusion tensor imaging (DTI), provide a potential tool for examining the role of cortical connectivity in the development of cognitive and brain development in greater detail. This method (see Box 1) provides information on directionality and regularity of myelinated fiber tracts (e.g. [23]). Until recently, few studies have linked brain connectivity measures with cognitive development, although indirect measures of white matter suggest regional development in prefrontal cortex and presumably in function [23]. Of these studies, one showed that development of working memory capacity was correlated positively with prefrontal–parietal connectivity [24], consistent with imaging studies showing differential recruitment of these regions in children relative to adults [39,42]. Using a similar approach, Liston and colleagues [25] showed that DTI-based connectivity in both frontostriatal and posterior fiber tracts correlated with age, but only frontostriatal connectivity correlated with efficient (fast and accurate) performance on a Go/No-go task. The prefrontal fiber tracts examined were defined by regions of interest identified in an fMRI study using the same task [42]. Similarly, combined DTI and fMRI analytical approaches have shown the importance of the maturation of prefrontal–parietal connectivity in the performance of a working memory task [26]. As such, DTI and fMRI-based measures with these regions correlated across age. In these studies, measures of brain connectivity were correlated with development, but specificity of the involvement of particular fiber tracts in cognitive performance was shown by dissociating the particular tract [25] or cognitive ability [24]. These dissociations are important when distinguishing between performance and agerelated differences in imaging measures. Functional organization of the developing human brain What do the previously described changes in brain structure, such as prolonged development of the prefrontal volume and connectivity, mean in terms of function? The development of the prefrontal cortex is thought to play an important role in the maturation of higher cognitive abilities [27,28]. Mature cognition is characterized by the ability to filter and suppress irrelevant information and actions (sensorimotor processes), in favor of relevant ones (i.e. cognitive control) [27]. A child’s capacity to filter information and suppress inappropriate actions in favor of appropriate ones continues to develop across the first two decades of life, with susceptibility to interference from competing sources lessening with maturity [29–32]. Many paradigms used to study cognitive development require cognitive control such as the Stroop and Go/No-go tasks [33–35]. Collectively, imaging studies using these tasks as probes show that children recruit distinct but often more prefrontal regions, both ventral and dorsal, when performing these tasks than do adults. Based on cross-sectional [36] and longitudinal studies [37], regions whose brain activity correlates with task performance (reaction time and/or accuracy) become more focal or fine-tuned, whereas regions not correlated with task performance decrease in activity with age (see Figure 1). It has been suggested that this pattern of activity, observed across a variety of paradigms, reflects development within, and refinement of, projections to and from these regions [28,30,34,36,38], consistent with the DTI findings presented previously. 106 Review TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.9 No.3 March 2005 www.sciencedirect.com
Review TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.9 No.3 March 2005 10> atchi and (iii)age-related As such,this approach provides a better underst anding performance-related neural recruitment 39,children prefrontal regions as those activated by adults. age-related ent in this region However,they reasing activatio arietal regions)consisten ctivated in adults Chil dren with p sugg ateng that impr mature activation of posterior parietal ecific. r cognitiv e demand lective show ag elated differences 41 As not app ametri var or ulus degradation),allowi ons dre on tria that the f Go trial sponses pre ding a no-g and adults had mo ra they as the pre ceding a No-go tria ine as five Go trials.The imaging data from adults mn asso ted withh a monotonic reas r of Go trial ding a no-go trial in d but en ance differer ces versus maturational differer ces n activation with age t n 11 five Go trials ing 39 that in ature cognitior this y an en eason the of avioral crucial e tiwitytointer cr lways pertorn ,specifically in prefrontal and posterior parietal-related circuitry het Cr ctional studies versus longitudinal studies Most of developmental observations T ed above this pted different udies (1.441.Ho or ever apar alyses might falsely sugges hanges over time or fail to 28,36,39,40 the ang ost hoc subj jects are divide into subgroups based on individuals.espe cially during development 6.7.15 matched study,Du coll
Performance differences versus maturational differences The region-specific differences in activation with age that have been reported could reflect maturation, but might also reflect simple performance differences. For this reason, the collection of behavioral responses is crucial across these studies. As children almost always perform worse than adults on higher cognitive tasks, without equating performance between age groups or controlling for performance differences it is difficult to specify whether the activation differences are age related or simply reflect an overall difference in performance. To address this issue, investigators have adopted different approaches for teasing apart age versus performancedriven differences. One key strategy is to use performance matching to equate behavioral performance [28,36,39,40]. Post hoc, subjects are divided into subgroups based on behavioral performance that is either matched across groups or not matched [40]. Three patterns of activation emerge from performance matching-based analyses: (i) performance/age-independent; (ii) performance-related; and (iii) age-related. These patterns help to identify the basis of the observed activation and/or regional differences and how they relate to the task demands. As such, this approach provides a better understanding of how maturation relates to increased cognitive abilities. For instance, in a study of cognitive control that showed performance-related neural recruitment [39], children were divided into ‘better and worse performers’. Children with effective response inhibition did not recruit the same prefrontal regions as those activated by adults, suggesting an age-related recruitment in this region. However, they did recruit a subset of the same posterior association areas (parietal regions) consistently activated in adults. Children with poor response inhibition did not recruit these posterior regions, suggesting that improved ability to withhold an inappropriate response might first require mature activation of posterior parietal regions that is task specific. Tasks of lesser cognitive demand (e.g. selective attention tasks without response competition) do not seem to show age-related differences [41]. As not all tasks yield comparable performance across age groups, a second approach that has been used to equate performance across groups is to manipulate task difficulty parametrically (e.g. [42,43]). In a parametric design, task difficulty is varied with increases in task demands (e.g. increased response competition, memory load or stimulus degradation), allowing comparisons between children and adults on trials equated for accuracy. Durston and colleagues [42] used a version of a Go/No-go task that parametrically manipulated the number of Go trials (responses) preceding a No-go trial (withhold response). Behaviorally, they showed that both children and adults had more errors as the number of responses preceding a No-go trial increased. Children, however, had as many errors for No-go trials following a single Go trial as adults had when a No-go trial followed as many as five Go trials. The imaging data from adults showed a monotonic increase in association areas of the ventral prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices as the number of Go trials preceding a No-go trial increased, but children activated these regions maximally regardless of whether they had to withhold a response after one, three or five Go trials (responses). These data are consistent with other reports showing [39] that immature cognition is characterized by an enhanced sensitivity to interference from competing sources (e.g. response competition) that coincides with immature association cortex, specifically in prefrontal and posterior parietal-related circuitry. Cross-sectional studies versus longitudinal studies Most of the developmental observations described above have relied on cross-sectional samples (e.g. [36]) or even comparisons across studies [1,44]. However, cross-sectional analyses might falsely suggest changes over time or fail to detect the specificity or magnitude of these changes, given the large individual variability in brain structure among individuals, especially during development [6,7,15]. In a recent study, Durston and colleagues [37] used fMRI to track cognitive and brain development from late Figure 1. The development of human cortical function, as measured by contemporary imaging methods, reflects fine-tuning of a diffuse network of neuroanatomical regions [28,36,37]. Collectively, developmental neuroimaging studies of cognitive control processes suggest a general pattern of increased recruitment of slow maturing prefrontal cortex (references depicted here in red), especially dorsolateral prefrontal cortex [30,35,36,40,43] and ventral prefrontal cortex [28,34,37,39,42], and decreased recruitment of lower level sensory regions (references in blue), including extrastriate [36,40] and fusiform cortex [40] and also posterior parietal areas [30,37,38]. Importantly, specific activations vary with task demands, so working memory [43] and Stroop tasks [35] recruit different regions from response–inhibition tasks [28,34,37,39,41,42]. This pattern of activity, which has been observed across a variety of paradigms, suggests that higher cognitive abilities supported by association cortex become more focal or fine-tuned with development, whereas other regions not specifically correlated with that specific cognitive ability become attenuated. AZanterior; PZposterior. Review TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.9 No.3 March 2005 107 www.sciencedirect.com
108 Review TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.9 No.3 March 2005 childhood into early adolescence during performance of a Box 3.Questions for future research Go/No-go task.There was a developmental shift in patterns of cortical activation from diffuse to focal activity Do the biological substrates of learning and development differ? similar to those reported in large cross-sectional studies On what timescale can we see neuroanatomical and physiological changes with learning and development? [36].Regions uncorrelated with task performance were What neural changes coincide with enhanced learning during recruited less with age,whereas a region in the ventral development that could hinder learning with maturity? prefrontal cortex that correlated with performance(speed How can we move beyond claims of causality between coinciden- and accuracy)in this study and prior studies [28,42,45,46] tal changes in brain and behavioral development with converging showed enhanced recruitment.This change in pattern of methods? Can typical human development provide important clues on the activity was associated with enhanced performance of the type and timing of interventions with atypical development? cognitive control task.Activation in earlier developing regions such as the primary motor cortex remained unchanged.A parallel cross-sectional analysis based on behavioral and cognitive interventions on developmental a second group of children,the same ages as the disorders such as dyslexia 51,52]and attention deficit- longitudinal group,showed less specific results,support- hyperactivity disorder [53]. ing the importance of longitudinal studies in evaluating Another future direction of developmental imaging cortical changes with age. studies will be in the combined use of complementary These studies,together with the longitudinal MRI- imaging methods.For example,a method not mentioned based morphometry studies [6],suggest differential in this review,but one that has been used to address developmental trajectories for sensorimotor relative to questions about cognitive and brain development,is association areas such as the prefrontal cortex.Both the electrophysiology.This technique records brain electrical imaging-based neuroanatomical studies and the func- activity that can be time-locked to presentation of a tional studies highlight the importance of examining stimulus or to the production of a response (i.e.event- changes in ability within individuals over time.As such, related potentials or ERPs).These electrophysiological this work can begin to delineate processes specific to measures have millisecond temporal resolution that can learning or development. provide important information on the developmental changes in the temporal characteristics of neural and Cortical organization with learning cognitive processes beyond that provided by fMRI [541. The importance of tracking cortical changes in individuals Clearly,the combination of these two techniques,together over time is perhaps most evident in the area of learning. with other methods such as DTI,will strongly enhance our Using repeated scanning of the same individuals,Karni ability to understand neural and cognitive development and others(e.g.[47,48])have shown rapid learning effects and constrain current developmental theories.Investi- in primary motor cortex of adults during motor sequence gators have begun to move in that direction with parallel learning that were apparent within a single imaging fMRI and DTI studies in adults and children [25.261. session,but that increased over weeks of training.This use of fMRI to trace learning-related changes in cortical Conclusions areas is currently being used by others in adults Findings from both cross-sectional and longitudinal (e.g.[49,50]).Across these studies,the pattern of results imaging studies of late childhood and adolescence show shows that activity in task-relevant regions becomes that brain regions associated with more basic functions increasingly enhanced with training,whereas task-irre- such as motor and sensory processes mature first,followed levant regions become less activated over time [50].This by association areas involved in top-down control of pattern of change during adult learning studies mimics thoughts and action.This pattern of development is the observed changes in cross-sectional [36]and longi- paralleled by a shift from diffuse to more focal recruitment tudinal [37]developmental studies.The findings highlight of cortical regions with learning and cognitive develop- the importance of examining and distinguishing between ment.Fine-tuning of cortical systems occurs with pro- contributions from experience and learning compared to tracted refinement of association cortex (e.g.prefrontal those associated more with maturational development,in cortex)relative to sensorimotor cortex.These findings driving the cortical patterns of activation observed. seem to parallel changes observed over shorter time periods in studies of adult learning.The reported shift in Future directions cortical architecture and function is presumably an Future research will no doubt take advantage of the experience-driven maturational process that reflects ability to image children multiple times with fMRI over fine-tuning of neural systems with experience and devel- the course of learning to delineate developmental and opment,but future work delineating how learning during experience-based processes in cortical activity(see Box 3). development affects this pattern is needed.These imaging For example,to determine whether the immature brain studies map onto findings from animal and human after extended practice engages in the same neural postmortem studies indicating that pruning and elimin- processes as the mature brain,we could compare brain ation of connections,in combination with strengthening of activity in the mature system with brain activity in the relevant ones,occur during this time period,and illustrate immature system both before and after extended experi- the subtle interplay between neuroanatomical and phys- ence.Investigators are already beginning to take advan- iological changes in neural circuitry and cognitive tage of this approach in investigating the impact of maturation. www.sciencedirect.com
childhood into early adolescence during performance of a Go/No-go task. There was a developmental shift in patterns of cortical activation from diffuse to focal activity similar to those reported in large cross-sectional studies [36]. Regions uncorrelated with task performance were recruited less with age, whereas a region in the ventral prefrontal cortex that correlated with performance (speed and accuracy) in this study and prior studies [28,42,45,46] showed enhanced recruitment. This change in pattern of activity was associated with enhanced performance of the cognitive control task. Activation in earlier developing regions such as the primary motor cortex remained unchanged. A parallel cross-sectional analysis based on a second group of children, the same ages as the longitudinal group, showed less specific results, supporting the importance of longitudinal studies in evaluating cortical changes with age. These studies, together with the longitudinal MRIbased morphometry studies [6], suggest differential developmental trajectories for sensorimotor relative to association areas such as the prefrontal cortex. Both the imaging-based neuroanatomical studies and the functional studies highlight the importance of examining changes in ability within individuals over time. As such, this work can begin to delineate processes specific to learning or development. Cortical organization with learning The importance of tracking cortical changes in individuals over time is perhaps most evident in the area of learning. Using repeated scanning of the same individuals, Karni and others (e.g. [47,48]) have shown rapid learning effects in primary motor cortex of adults during motor sequence learning that were apparent within a single imaging session, but that increased over weeks of training. This use of fMRI to trace learning-related changes in cortical areas is currently being used by others in adults (e.g. [49,50]). Across these studies, the pattern of results shows that activity in task-relevant regions becomes increasingly enhanced with training, whereas task-irrelevant regions become less activated over time [50]. This pattern of change during adult learning studies mimics the observed changes in cross-sectional [36] and longitudinal [37] developmental studies. The findings highlight the importance of examining and distinguishing between contributions from experience and learning compared to those associated more with maturational development, in driving the cortical patterns of activation observed. Future directions Future research will no doubt take advantage of the ability to image children multiple times with fMRI over the course of learning to delineate developmental and experience-based processes in cortical activity (see Box 3). For example, to determine whether the immature brain after extended practice engages in the same neural processes as the mature brain, we could compare brain activity in the mature system with brain activity in the immature system both before and after extended experience. Investigators are already beginning to take advantage of this approach in investigating the impact of behavioral and cognitive interventions on developmental disorders such as dyslexia [51,52] and attention deficit– hyperactivity disorder [53]. Another future direction of developmental imaging studies will be in the combined use of complementary imaging methods. For example, a method not mentioned in this review, but one that has been used to address questions about cognitive and brain development, is electrophysiology. This technique records brain electrical activity that can be time-locked to presentation of a stimulus or to the production of a response (i.e. eventrelated potentials or ERPs). These electrophysiological measures have millisecond temporal resolution that can provide important information on the developmental changes in the temporal characteristics of neural and cognitive processes beyond that provided by fMRI [54]. Clearly, the combination of these two techniques, together with other methods such as DTI, will strongly enhance our ability to understand neural and cognitive development and constrain current developmental theories. Investigators have begun to move in that direction with parallel fMRI and DTI studies in adults and children [25,26]. Conclusions Findings from both cross-sectional and longitudinal imaging studies of late childhood and adolescence show that brain regions associated with more basic functions such as motor and sensory processes mature first, followed by association areas involved in top-down control of thoughts and action. This pattern of development is paralleled by a shift from diffuse to more focal recruitment of cortical regions with learning and cognitive development. Fine-tuning of cortical systems occurs with protracted refinement of association cortex (e.g. prefrontal cortex) relative to sensorimotor cortex. These findings seem to parallel changes observed over shorter time periods in studies of adult learning. The reported shift in cortical architecture and function is presumably an experience-driven maturational process that reflects fine-tuning of neural systems with experience and development, but future work delineating how learning during development affects this pattern is needed. These imaging studies map onto findings from animal and human postmortem studies indicating that pruning and elimination of connections, in combination with strengthening of relevant ones, occur during this time period, and illustrate the subtle interplay between neuroanatomical and physiological changes in neural circuitry and cognitive maturation. Box 3. Questions for future research † Do the biological substrates of learning and development differ? † On what timescale can we see neuroanatomical and physiological changes with learning and development? † What neural changes coincide with enhanced learning during development that could hinder learning with maturity? † How can we move beyond claims of causality between coincidental changes in brain and behavioral development with converging methods? † Can typical human development provide important clues on the type and timing of interventions with atypical development? 108 Review TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.9 No.3 March 2005 www.sciencedirect.com
Review TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.9 No.3 March 2005 109 Acknowledgements 28 Casey,B.J.et al.(1997)A developmental functional MRI study of This work was supported in part by P0l MH62196,R01 MH63255 and prefrontal activation during performance of a Go/No-go task.Cogn. R21 DA15882 to B.J.C. Neurosci.9,835-847 29 Diamond,A.(1996)Evidence for the importance of dopamine for References prefrontal cortex functions early in life.Philos.Trans.R.Soc.London B Biol..Sci.351,1483-1493 1 Casey,B.J.et al.(1995)Activation of prefrontal cortex in children during a nonspatial working memory task with functional MRI. 30 Casey,B.J.et al.(2002)Dissociating striatal and hippocampal function developmentally with a stimulus-response compatibility Neuroimage 2,221-229 task.J.Neurosci.22,8647-8652 2 Casey,B.J.et al.(2000)Structural and functional brain development 31 Goldman-Rakic,P.S.(1987)Development of cortical circuitry and and its relation to cognitive development.Biol.Psychol.54,241-257 3 Spear,L.P.(2000)The adolescent brain and age-related behavioral cognitive function.Child Dev.58.601-622 32 Munakata,Y.and Yerys,B.E.(2001)All together now:when manifestations.Neurosci.Biobehav.Rev.24,417-463 4 Sowell,E.R.et al.(2001)Improved memory functioning and frontal dissociations between knowledge and action disappear.Psychol.Sci. 12,335-337 lobe maturation between childhood and adolescence:a structural MRI study.J.Int.Neuropsychol.Soc.7,312-322 33 Casey,B.J.et al.(2000)Structural and functional brain development 5 Durston,S.et al.(2001)Anatomical MRI of the developing human and its relation to cognitive development.Biol.Psychol.54,241-257 brain:What have we learned?J.Am.Acad.Child Adolese.Psychiatry 34 Tamm,L.et al.(2002)Maturation of brain function associated with 40,1012-1020 response inhibition.J.Am.Acad.Child Adolesc.Psychiatry 41, 6 Gogtay,N.et al.(2004)Dynamic mapping of human cortical 1231-1238 development during childhood through early adulthood.Proc.Natl. 35 Adelman,N.E.et al.(2002)A Developmental fMRI study of the Stroop Acad.Sci.U.S.A.101,8174-8179 color-word task.Neuroimage 16,61-75 7 Sowell,E.R.et al.(2004)Longitudinal mapping of cortical thickness 36 Brown,T.T.et al.Developmental changes in human cerebral and brain growth in normal children.J.Neurosci.24.8223-8231 functional organization for word generation.Cereb.Cortex (in 8 Giedd,J.N.(2004)Structural magnetic resonance imaging of the press) adolescent brain.Ann.N.Y.Acad.Sci.1021,77-85 37 Durston,S.et al.(2004)Longitudinal functional MRI of the 9 Sowell,E.R.et al.(2003)Mapping cortical change across the human development of cognitive control.Soc.Neurosci.Abstr:319.18 life span.Nat.Neurosci.6,309-315 38 Thomas.K.M.et al.(2004)Evidence of developmental differences in 10 Reiss,A.L.et al.(1996)Brain development,gender and IQ in children. implicit sequence learning:an fMRI study of children and adults. A volumetric imaging study.Brain 119.1763-1774 J.Cogn.Neurosci.16,1339-1351 11 Huttenlocher,P.R.(1979)Synaptic density in human frontal cortex: 39 Bunge,S.A.et al.(2002)Immature frontal lobe contributions to Developmental changes and effects of aging.Brain Res.163,195-205 cognitive control in children:evidence from fMRI.Neuron 33,301-311 12 Bourgeois,J.P.et al.(1994)Synaptogenesis in the prefrontal cortex of 40 Schlaggar,B.L.et al.(2002)Functional neuroanatomical differences rhesus monkeys.Cereb.Cortex 4,78-96 between adults and school-age children in the processing of single 13 Jernigan,T.L.et al.(1991)Magnetic resonance imaging abnormalities words.Science 296,1476-1479 in lenticular nuclei and cerebral cortex in schizophrenia.Arch.Gen. 41 Booth,J.R.et al.(2003)Neural development of selective attention and Psychiatry 48,881-890 response inhibition.Neuroimage 20,737-751 14 Pfefferbaum,A.et al.(1994)A quantitative magnetic resonance 42 Durston,S.et al.(2002)A neural basis for the development of imaging study of changes in brain morphology from infancy to late inhibitory control.Dev.Sei.5,F9-F16 adulthood.Arch.Neurol.51,874-887 43 Klingberg,T.et al.(2002)Increased brain activity in frontal and 15 Caviness,V.S.et al.(1996)The human brain age 7-11 years:a parietal cortex underlies the development of visuospatial working volumetric analysis based on magnetic resonance images.Cereb. memory capacity during childhood.J.Cogn.Neurosci.14,1-10 Cortex6,726-736 44 Cohen.J.D.et al.(1994)Activation ofprefrontal cortex in a non spatial 16 Giedd,J.N.et al.(1996)Quantitative magnetic resonance imaging of working memory task with functional MRI.Hum.Brain Mapp.1, human brain development:ages 4-18.Cereb.Cortex 6,551-560 293-304 17 Giedd,J.N.et al.(1999)Brain development during childhood and 45 Konishi,S.et al.(1999)Common inhibitory mechanism in human adolescence:a longitudinal MRI study.Nat.Neurosci.10,861-863 inferior prefrontal cortex revealed by event-related functional MRI 18 Sowell,E.R.et al.(2003)Mapping cortical change across the human Brain122,981-991 life span.Nat.Neurosci.6,309-315 46 Konishi,S.et al.(1998)No-go dominant brain activity in human 19 Sowell,E.R.et al.(1999)In vivo evidence for post-adolescent inferior prefrontal cortex revealed by functional magnetic resonance brain maturation in frontal and striatal regions.Nat.Neurosci.2, imaging.Eur.J.Neurosci.10,1209-1213 859-861 20 Paus.T.et al.(1999)Structural maturation of neural pathways in 47 Karni,A.et al.(1998)The acquisition of skilled motor performance: children and adolescents:in vivo study.Science 283,1908-1911 fast and slow experience-driven changes in primary motor cortex Proc.Natl.Acad.Sci.U.S.A.95.861-868 21 Casey,B.J.et al.(1997)Implication of right frontostriatal circuitry in response inhibition and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder 48 Karni,A.et al.(1995)Functional MRI evidence for adult motor cortex plasticity during motor skill learning.Nature 377.155-158 J.Am.Acad.Child Adolese.Psychiatry 36,374-383 22 Casey,B.J.et al.(1997)The role of the anterior cingulate in automatic 49 Green,C.S.and Bavelier,D.(2003)Action video game modifies visual and controlled processes:A developmental neuroanatomical study. selective attention.Nature 423,534-537 Dev.Psychobiol.30.61-69 50 Olesen,P.J.et al.(2004)Increased prefrontal and parietal activity 23 Klingberg,T.et al.(1999)Myelination and organization of the frontal after training of working memory.Nat.Neurosci.7,75-79 white matter in children:a diffusion tensor MRI study.Neuroreport 51 McCandliss,B.D.and Noble,K.G.(2003)The development of reading 10,2817-2821 impairment:a cognitive neuroscience model.Ment.Retard.Dev. 24 Nagy,Z.et al.(2004)Maturation of white matter is associated with the Disabil.Res.Rev.9,196-204 development of cognitive functions during childhood.J.Cogn 52 Temple,E.(2002)Brain mechanisms in normal and dyslexic readers. Neurosci..16,1227-1233 Curr.Opin.Neurobiol.12,178-183 25 Liston,C.et al.(2003)Developmental differences in diffusion 53 Klingberg,T.et al.(2002)Training of working memory in children measures of cortical fiber tracts.J.Cogn.Neurosci.15,S57-S58 with ADHD.J.Clin.Exp.Neuropsychol.24,781-791 26 Olesen,P.J.et al.(2003)Combined analysis of DTI and fMRI data 54 Casey,B.J.and de Haan,M.(2002)Introduction:new methods in reveals a joint maturation of white and grey matter in a fronto- developmental science.Dev.Sci.5,265-267 parietal network.Brain Res.Cogn.Brain Res.18,48-57 55 Logothetis,N.K.et al.(2001)Neurophysiological investigation of the 27 Casey,B.J.et al.(2002)Clinical,imaging,lesion,and genetic basis of the fMRI signal.Nature 412,150-157 approaches toward a model of cognitive control.Dev.Psychobiol.40. 56 Bandettini,P.A.and Ungerleider,L.G.(2001)From neuron to BOLD: 237-254 new connections.Nat.Neurosci.4,864-866 www.sciencedirect.com
Acknowledgements This work was supported in part by P01 MH62196, R01 MH63255 and R21 DA15882 to B.J.C. References 1 Casey, B.J. et al. (1995) Activation of prefrontal cortex in children during a nonspatial working memory task with functional MRI. Neuroimage 2, 221–229 2 Casey, B.J. et al. (2000) Structural and functional brain development and its relation to cognitive development. Biol. Psychol. 54, 241–257 3 Spear, L.P. (2000) The adolescent brain and age-related behavioral manifestations. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 24, 417–463 4 Sowell, E.R. et al. (2001) Improved memory functioning and frontal lobe maturation between childhood and adolescence: a structural MRI study. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 7, 312–322 5 Durston, S. et al. (2001) Anatomical MRI of the developing human brain: What have we learned? J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 40, 1012–1020 6 Gogtay, N. et al. (2004) Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 101, 8174–8179 7 Sowell, E.R. et al. (2004) Longitudinal mapping of cortical thickness and brain growth in normal children. J. Neurosci. 24, 8223–8231 8 Giedd, J.N. (2004) Structural magnetic resonance imaging of the adolescent brain. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1021, 77–85 9 Sowell, E.R. et al. (2003) Mapping cortical change across the human life span. Nat. Neurosci. 6, 309–315 10 Reiss, A.L. et al. (1996) Brain development, gender and IQ in children. A volumetric imaging study. Brain 119, 1763–1774 11 Huttenlocher, P.R. (1979) Synaptic density in human frontal cortex: Developmental changes and effects of aging. Brain Res. 163, 195–205 12 Bourgeois, J.P. et al. (1994) Synaptogenesis in the prefrontal cortex of rhesus monkeys. Cereb. Cortex 4, 78–96 13 Jernigan, T.L. et al. (1991) Magnetic resonance imaging abnormalities in lenticular nuclei and cerebral cortex in schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 48, 881–890 14 Pfefferbaum, A. et al. (1994) A quantitative magnetic resonance imaging study of changes in brain morphology from infancy to late adulthood. Arch. Neurol. 51, 874–887 15 Caviness, V.S. et al. (1996) The human brain age 7–11 years: a volumetric analysis based on magnetic resonance images. Cereb. Cortex 6, 726–736 16 Giedd, J.N. et al. (1996) Quantitative magnetic resonance imaging of human brain development: ages 4–18. Cereb. Cortex 6, 551–560 17 Giedd, J.N. et al. (1999) Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study. Nat. Neurosci. 10, 861–863 18 Sowell, E.R. et al. (2003) Mapping cortical change across the human life span. Nat. Neurosci. 6, 309–315 19 Sowell, E.R. et al. (1999) In vivo evidence for post-adolescent brain maturation in frontal and striatal regions. Nat. Neurosci. 2, 859–861 20 Paus, T. et al. (1999) Structural maturation of neural pathways in children and adolescents: in vivo study. Science 283, 1908–1911 21 Casey, B.J. et al. (1997) Implication of right frontostriatal circuitry in response inhibition and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 36, 374–383 22 Casey, B.J. et al. (1997) The role of the anterior cingulate in automatic and controlled processes: A developmental neuroanatomical study. Dev. Psychobiol. 30, 61–69 23 Klingberg, T. et al. (1999) Myelination and organization of the frontal white matter in children: a diffusion tensor MRI study. Neuroreport 10, 2817–2821 24 Nagy, Z. et al. (2004) Maturation of white matter is associated with the development of cognitive functions during childhood. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 1227–1233 25 Liston, C. et al. (2003) Developmental differences in diffusion measures of cortical fiber tracts. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 15, S57–S58 26 Olesen, P.J. et al. (2003) Combined analysis of DTI and fMRI data reveals a joint maturation of white and grey matter in a frontoparietal network. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 18, 48–57 27 Casey, B.J. et al. (2002) Clinical, imaging, lesion, and genetic approaches toward a model of cognitive control. Dev. Psychobiol. 40, 237–254 28 Casey, B.J. et al. (1997) A developmental functional MRI study of prefrontal activation during performance of a Go/No-go task. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 9, 835–847 29 Diamond, A. (1996) Evidence for the importance of dopamine for prefrontal cortex functions early in life. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. London B Biol. Sci. 351, 1483–1493 30 Casey, B.J. et al. (2002) Dissociating striatal and hippocampal function developmentally with a stimulus–response compatibility task. J. Neurosci. 22, 8647–8652 31 Goldman-Rakic, P.S. (1987) Development of cortical circuitry and cognitive function. Child Dev. 58, 601–622 32 Munakata, Y. and Yerys, B.E. (2001) All together now: when dissociations between knowledge and action disappear. Psychol. Sci. 12, 335–337 33 Casey, B.J. et al. (2000) Structural and functional brain development and its relation to cognitive development. Biol. Psychol. 54, 241–257 34 Tamm, L. et al. (2002) Maturation of brain function associated with response inhibition. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 41, 1231–1238 35 Adelman, N.E. et al. (2002) A Developmental fMRI study of the Stroop color–word task. Neuroimage 16, 61–75 36 Brown, T.T. et al. Developmental changes in human cerebral functional organization for word generation. Cereb. Cortex (in press) 37 Durston, S. et al. (2004) Longitudinal functional MRI of the development of cognitive control. Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 319.18 38 Thomas, K.M. et al. (2004) Evidence of developmental differences in implicit sequence learning: an fMRI study of children and adults. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 1339–1351 39 Bunge, S.A. et al. (2002) Immature frontal lobe contributions to cognitive control in children: evidence from fMRI. Neuron 33, 301–311 40 Schlaggar, B.L. et al. (2002) Functional neuroanatomical differences between adults and school-age children in the processing of single words. Science 296, 1476–1479 41 Booth, J.R. et al. (2003) Neural development of selective attention and response inhibition. Neuroimage 20, 737–751 42 Durston, S. et al. (2002) A neural basis for the development of inhibitory control. Dev. Sci. 5, F9–F16 43 Klingberg, T. et al. (2002) Increased brain activity in frontal and parietal cortex underlies the development of visuospatial working memory capacity during childhood. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 14, 1–10 44 Cohen, J.D. et al. (1994) Activation of prefrontal cortex in a non spatial working memory task with functional MRI. Hum. Brain Mapp. 1, 293–304 45 Konishi, S. et al. (1999) Common inhibitory mechanism in human inferior prefrontal cortex revealed by event-related functional MRI. Brain 122, 981–991 46 Konishi, S. et al. (1998) No-go dominant brain activity in human inferior prefrontal cortex revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Eur. J. Neurosci. 10, 1209–1213 47 Karni, A. et al. (1998) The acquisition of skilled motor performance: fast and slow experience-driven changes in primary motor cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 95, 861–868 48 Karni, A. et al. (1995) Functional MRI evidence for adult motor cortex plasticity during motor skill learning. Nature 377, 155–158 49 Green, C.S. and Bavelier, D. (2003) Action video game modifies visual selective attention. Nature 423, 534–537 50 Olesen, P.J. et al. (2004) Increased prefrontal and parietal activity after training of working memory. Nat. Neurosci. 7, 75–79 51 McCandliss, B.D. and Noble, K.G. (2003) The development of reading impairment: a cognitive neuroscience model. Ment. Retard. Dev. Disabil. Res. Rev. 9, 196–204 52 Temple, E. (2002) Brain mechanisms in normal and dyslexic readers. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 12, 178–183 53 Klingberg, T. et al. (2002) Training of working memory in children with ADHD. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 24, 781–791 54 Casey, B.J. and de Haan, M. (2002) Introduction: new methods in developmental science. Dev. Sci. 5, 265–267 55 Logothetis, N.K. et al. (2001) Neurophysiological investigation of the basis of the fMRI signal. Nature 412, 150–157 56 Bandettini, P.A. and Ungerleider, L.G. (2001) From neuron to BOLD: new connections. Nat. Neurosci. 4, 864–866 Review TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.9 No.3 March 2005 109 www.sciencedirect.com
10 Review TRENDS in Cognitive Scienc Vol.9 No.3 March 2005 57 Kwong.K.K.et al.(1992)Dynamic Med.14.68 78 a28.517-52 、宗“文改 ,A96 enetic cyclesof aging of the huma the Br 。 Five things you might not know about Elsevier 2 Haveyou contributed toan Elsevierjo wthat all our authors areentitled toa30%discount on books and stand- 4 er has yyears nas alwebsitesandinStitUtionalrepositor omen's ybo on Confe n of the S irect.com
57 Kwong, K.K. et al. (1992) Dynamic magnetic resonance imaging of human brain activity during primary sensory stimulation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 89, 5675–5679 58 Ogawa, S. et al. (1990) Oxygenation-sensitive contrast in magnetic resonance image of rodent brain at high magnetic fields. Magn. Reson. Med. 14, 68–78 59 Le Bihan, D. (2003) Looking into the functional architecture of the brain with diffusion MRI. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 4, 469–480 60 Pierpaoli, C. et al. (1996) Diffusion tensor MR imaging of the human brain. Radiology 201, 637–648 61 Conel, J.L. (1939–1963) The Postnatal Development of the Human Cerebral Cortex (Vols 1–6), Harvard University Press 62 Rabinowicz, T. (1986) The differentiated maturation of the cerebral cortex. In Human Growth (Falkner, F. and Tanner, J.M., eds), pp. 385–410, Plenum Press 63 Jacobson, M. (1991) Developmental Neurobiology, Plenum Press 64 Huttenlocher, P.R. (1990) Morphometric study of human cerebral cortex development. Neuropsychologia 28, 517–527 65 Yakovlev, P.I. and Lecours, A. (1967) The myelogenetic cycles of regional maturation in the brain. In Regional Development of the Brain in Early Life (Minkowski, A., ed.), pp. 3–65, Blackwell 66 Thompson, R.A. and Nelson, C.A. (2001) Developmental science and the media. Early brain development. Am. Psychol. 56, 5–15 Five things you might not know about Elsevier 1. Elsevier is a founder member of the WHO’s HINARI and AGORA initiatives, which enable the world’s poorest countries to gain free access to scientific literature. More than 1000 journals, including the Trends and Current Opinion collections, will be available for free or at significantly reduced prices. 2. The online archive of Elsevier’s premier Cell Press journal collection will become freely available from January 2005. Free access to the recent archive, including Cell, Neuron, Immunity and Current Biology, will be available on both ScienceDirect and the Cell Press journal sites 12 months after articles are first published. 3. Have you contributed to an Elsevier journal, book or series? Did you know that all our authors are entitled to a 30% discount on books and stand-alone CDs when ordered directly from us? For more information, call our sales offices: +1 800 782 4927 (US) or +1 800 460 3110 (Canada, South & Central America) or +44 1865 474 010 (rest of the world) 4. Elsevier has a long tradition of liberal copyright policies and for many years has permitted both the posting of preprints on public servers and the posting of final papers on internal servers. Now, Elsevier has extended its author posting policy to allow authors to freely post the final text version of their papers on both their personal websites and institutional repositories or websites. 5. The Elsevier Foundation is a knowledge-centered foundation making grants and contributions throughout the world. A reflection of our culturally rich global organization, the Foundation has funded, for example, the setting up of a video library to educate for children in Philadelphia, provided storybooks to children in Cape Town, sponsored the creation of the Stanley L. Robbins Visiting Professorship at Brigham and Women’s Hospital and given funding to the 3rd International Conference on Children’s Health and the Environment. 110 Review TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.9 No.3 March 2005 www.sciencedirect.com