当前位置:高等教育资讯网  >  中国高校课件下载中心  >  大学文库  >  浏览文档

《社会心理学》课程教学资源(文献资料)group processes——Social Loafing and Social Compensation - The Effects of Expectations of Co-Worker Performance

资源类别:文库,文档格式:PDF,文档页数:12,文件大小:1.52MB,团购合买
点击下载完整版文档(PDF)

Nchloy Copyright199句the Am n Pchol Social Loafing and Social Compensation:The Effects of Expectations of Co-Worker Performance Kigigbowa ere Unkery reh ha ted that people tendto ei orking colle S,d oasHakdp982,oreieheaie2opcohcahc mittees, sports teams s.quality contr le of a teacher who divides a class int c5hacobinethetersoomasneepndeLpG togeth r on a project cited in K ,1986nthc1880 y on a rope pulling ask.A ompared with a situation n whi of social lective tas sks (see ackson&Wil ms,1989) loafing in his le s,and studentsre to moderate ocial loafing can be as an e ample of students des ring to p them ced or by asing the identifiability or e in a la It is als ly,ho Harkins.1987:Williams.Harkins.).enhanc certain cire most will feel it n y to for othe ch thi too long Portionsof this article w ted att e1988 Nags Head Confe done the pr requisite course g bac in Chicag: Gardner,Katherine Gad.Katbe ne Kerr We also thank Rob an in ant role in s ice Kelly Norbert Ke by de nt of or reques X0434 570

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 1991, Vol. 61, No. 4,570-581 Copyright 1991 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0022-3514/91/S3.00 Social Loafing and Social Compensation: The Effects of Expectations of Co-Worker Performance Kipling D. Williams University of Toledo Steven J. Karau Purdue University Previous research has suggested that people tend to engage in social loafing when working collec￾tively. The present research tested the social compensation hypothesis, which states that people will work harder collectively than individually when they expect their co-workers to perform poorly on a meaningful task. In 3 experiments, participants worked either collectively or coactively on an idea generation task. Expectations of co-worker performance were either inferred from participants' interpersonal trust scores (Experiment 1) or were directly manipulated by a confederate coworker's statement of either his intended effort (Experiment 2) or his ability at the task (Experiment 3). All 3 studies supported the social compensation hypothesis. Additionally, Experiment 3 supported the hypothesis that participants would not socially compensate for a poorly performing co-worker when working on a task that was low in meaningfulness. People often work together in groups to accomplish various goals. Many group tasks are collective tasks, in which members' contributions are pooled with those of their co-workers. Com￾mittees, sports teams, juries, marching bands, quality control teams, and government task forces are but a few examples of groups that combine their efforts to form a single product. Possi￾bly the first social psychological study, conducted in the 1880s by Ringelmann (cited in Kravitz & Martin, 1986), examined the effects of working collectively on a rope pulling task. At present, more than 55 studies have examined the effort ex￾pended by individuals when working on individual versus col￾lective tasks (see Jackson & Williams, 1989). The results of these studies indicate that people expend less effort collectively than coactively (working individually but in the presence of other co-workers), a phenomenon referred to as social loafing. A number of variables have been demonstrated to moderate social loafing. For example, social loafing can be reduced or eliminated by increasing the identifiability or eva￾luability of the individual members' contributions' (Szymanski & Harkins, 1987; Williams, Harkins, & Latane, 1981), enhanc￾Portions of this article were presented at the 1988 Nags Head Confer￾ence on Groups and Organizations; the 1988 96th Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association convention in Atlanta, Georgia; the 1989 Midwestern Psychological Association convention in Chicago; and the 1990 Society for Experimental Social Psychology convention in Buffalo, New \brk. We thank Robert Flaherty, Lynn Gardner, Katherine Glad, Kather￾ine Kerr, Doug Langon, Jon Stroud, and Brad Thurmond for their assistance in running the experiments. We also thank Robert Arkin, Cathy Booth, Martin Bourgeois, Stephen Harkins, Irwin Horowitz, Jeffrey Jackson, Amy Kaylor, Janice Kelly, Norbert Kerr, Constantine Sedikides, and James Shepperd for their comments on this article or on presentations of this research. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Kipling D. Williams, Department of Psychology, University of To￾ledo, Toledo, Ohio 43606, or requested through Internet: FAX0434 @UOFT02.TOLEDO.EDU. ing personal involvement with the task (Brickner, Harkins, & Ostrom, 1986), elevating the uniqueness of individual contribu￾tions (Harkins & Petty, 1982), or strengthening group cohesive￾ness (Williams, 1981). Consider an example of a teacher who divides a class into small groups and assigns them to work together on a project, hand in one paper, and share the grade. The social loafing litera￾ture would lead one to expect that each student's motivation and effort would decrease compared with a situation in which the teacher assigned individual papers and projects. In fact, the first author often uses this example as an illustration of social loafing in his lectures, and students remember how awful those types of assignments were because the "others in their groups always loafed." Ironically, and almost without exception, the students claimed that they were the ones left to "carry the load" for the others in their group. Of course, it is easy to view this response as an example of students desiring to present them￾selves in a favorable light. It is also likely, however, that certain people do exert more effort on collective tasks or that, under certain circumstances, most people will feel it necessary to shoulder the burden for others. Not too long ago in a class in which this type of assignment was given, a student who had done exceptionally well in the prerequisite course approached the instructor after class and, fighting back tears, stated that she knew she would end up doing all the work for the others in her 1 Some researchers (Harkins, 1987; Harkins & Szymanski, 1989; Kerr & Bruun, 1983) have defined social loafing as motivation loss in groups caused by reduced identifiability or evaluation. We agree that evaluation plays an important role in social loafing but choose not to define social loafing in these terms because (a) defining the phenome￾non in terms of its causes prevents, by definition, the discovery of new causes for the same effect; and (b) evidence suggests there are other causes of social loafing, such as redundancy of contribution (Harkins & Petty, 1982) and dispensability of effort (Kerr & Bruun, 1983). We prefer to adopt a less restrictive definition of social loafing consistent with the original formulation: the tendency to reduce one's effort when working collectively compared with individually on the same task. 570

SOCIAL LOAFING AND SOCIAL COMPENSATION 571 group.She felt that sheco oaf they with an expectancy-value framework ifindi believed that her orkers were probably not as moti ted or the ad n e but to do e on the ta to lead to highly valued ou s,ind ite att empts to ass ure her this did not ha sically me ngful or a with the evah a all of th of the group e by the e ital or hi order for for the ot up am cur.It is ely,h er,that individua On produce soc ompensation is the expectation that wes are p ose that ome pers et tha to the col ve goal to compe fo the the d sire for could ead pe ople to comper ns of t or nle de ck of tr rm wel ost re used t th its that ties are the lves with ot on is that the gro maduct is ins ant to the indiv idual If the task the group produc uggests tha bec of the e also Cialdini t al 1976).Goetha d Da rod s not matter to th 1987 stated then he or she will be motivated to avoid a poor hen (p.33).Presum a negati ial id group per nance by compensating for the poorly performing These hyr rigatbothwih xpectancy-value d be to k harder.that is.to 107 heir 3 p the 1964)suggest that individuals will be willing to exer man canno be eval d.Harkins an ieve th is heing Applied to social loafing.we would expe an ind he perfo ch the dy individu will h the orn by of c is d as t to self hen orker ce indi h K (or e tas rker perform well.the conti of the oth rs is expe ed to be good,then individuals can opt to nd the roup ev inputs.Hou when individuals expect their c rkers t Both of these theoretical pe ctives also explain why socia orm poorly,the co and the instrumentalit of perfo ance to ou ohe cy be an people are othe than individu ofother they expect their co-w rkers to perform well. mbers nd attributes of the task rm well ma not be sufficient to i motivation.Indi all of the s ten th the ith this poir h ggests tha nance onk

SOCIAL LOAFING AND SOCIAL COMPENSATION 571 group. She felt that she could not rely on the others because they would take advantage of the collective task and loaf. She also believed that her co-workers were probably not as motivated or able as she. She felt she had no choice but to do everything that the group was supposed to do to merely share an A on the paper. Despite attempts to assure her that this did not have to happen, she ended up doing almost all of the work. Why? We are proposing that under some conditions, people may actually work harder in a collective setting than in a coactive setting in order to compensate for the others in their group, an effect we refer to as social compensation. One factor that might produce social compensation is the expectation that other group members are performing insufficiently. Under these cir￾cumstances, we propose that individuals will feel compelled to contribute more to the collective goal to compensate for the inadequate contributions of their co-workers. The perception of inadequate co-worker contributions may be derived from a general lack of trust in the reliability of others to perform well when their contributions are pooled with those of others or from direct knowledge of co-workers' insufficient efforts or abil￾ities. The second factor we believe may be necessary to produce compensation is that the group product is in some way impor￾tant to the individuals involved. If the task or the group product is seen as meaningless, then there is no need to compensate for poorly performing co-workers, because the evaluation of the group product simply does not matter to the individual. If, how￾ever, the evaluation of the group product is important to the individual, then he or she will be motivated to avoid a poor group performance by compensating for the poorly performing co-workers. These hypotheses are consistent both with expectancy-value models of effort and with self-validation theories. For example, expectancy-value models of effort (e.g., Heckhausen, 1977; Vroom, 1964) suggest that individuals will be willing to exert effort on a task only to the degree that they expect their effort to result in performance that is instrumental in obtaining valued outcomes. Applied to social loafing, we would expect an indi￾vidual's motivation and effort to be greatest when working on a task in which the group performance is strongly contingent on his or her individual effort and when group performance is expected to lead to outcomes that are highly valued by the indi￾vidual (e.g., a favorable evaluation). Thus, expectations of co￾worker performance are likely to strongly influence individual motivation on collective tasks. When individuals expect their co-workers to perform well, the contingency between their own effort and the group performance is reduced because the group is likely to perform well even if the individuals reduce their own inputs. However, when individuals expect their co-workers to perform poorly, the contingency between their own effort and the group performance is actually enhanced. Thus, when out￾come valence and the instrumentality of performance to out￾comes are held constant, people are likely to work harder when they expect their co-workers to perform poorly and less hard when they expect their co-workers to perform well. However, the mere expectation that one's co-workers will not perform well may not be sufficient to increase motivation. Indi￾viduals will be concerned with the quality of the group perfor￾mance only when it is instrumental to obtaining valued out￾comes. In accord with an expectancy-value framework, if indi￾viduals expect their co-workers to perform poorly, they will not be willing to compensate unless they expect their performance on the task to lead to highly valued outcomes. Thus, individuals are more likely to compensate when they find the task to be intrinsically meaningful or are concerned with the evaluation of the group's performance by the experimenter, themselves, or co-workers. We do not mean to imply that the task must be viewed as vital or highly meaningful in order for compensation to occur. It is likely, however, that individuals will be unwilling to compensate for poorly performing co-workers if they expect their performance on the task to be irrelevant to obtaining an outcome that has at least some personal value. Theoretical orientations focusing on the self also suggest that the desire for self-validation could lead people to compensate for co-workers on a collective task. Several theories suggest that people desire positive evaluation, especially in comparison with others. Most research has focused on social comparison at the individual level, but Goethals and Darley's (1987) revision of social comparison theory suggests that people not only com￾pare themselves with others for self-validation, but they also compare the groups to which they belong with other groups for the same reasons. Similarly, social identity theory (Tajfel & Turner, 1986) suggests that often people gain positive self-iden￾tity through the accomplishments of the groups to which they belong (see also Cialdini et al., 1976). Goethals and Darley (1987) stated that "individuals experiencing negative social identity are predicted to leave their groups or attempt to make them better" (p. 33). Presumably, a negative social identity could be anticipated if one is performing on a collective task with unproductive co-workers. One way to make the group bet￾ter would be to work harder, that is, to compensate. Consistent with the notion that people are concerned for their group's evaluation even when their individual perfor￾mances cannot be evaluated, Harkins and Szymanski (1989) found that on collective tasks people are less likely to loaf if they believe the performance of their group is being compared with the performance of other groups. In the present context, when working on a collective task with others who are expected to perform poorly, individuals will be motivated to bolster the performance of their group by increasing their own efforts. This would be true only to the extent that performance on the task is perceived as pertinent to self-validation, hence, only when the task (or the evaluation of the task performance) is perceived as meaningful. On the other hand, if the performance of the others is expected to be good, then individuals can opt to reduce their efforts and still receive a favorable group evalua￾tion. Both of these theoretical perspectives also explain why social compensation has not emerged in previous research. According to the expectancy-value model, loafing occurs because there is a higher contingency between individual effort and valued out￾comes coactively than collectively. As group size increases, fac￾tors other than individual effort (e.g., the effort of other group members and attributes of the task) increasingly determine group performance, and valued outcomes (e.g., a favorable evalu￾ation) are diffused among all of the group members. Consistent with this point, the self-validation approach suggests that peo￾ple can more effectively control their evaluations when working

572 KIPLING D.WILLIAMS AND STEVEN J.KARAU social loafing effect was eliminated and that participants llective tasks,indi ched th ent the when the rker's statement of her intended effort wa co-workers to perform of the not expe heir co- h ught the interes and that she was that he thought the s boring and that she ot o to try hard. Hackman and Morris sug- eason for them to believe oorly and only if task willt it that atttude kers and se the n to they care out the evaluatic ectations of co- orker n motivations to ard their task are highly infuenced by co er studics that have manipulatc rkers ass nts of the ta he c ther and with our Kerr (1983) studied ual effort on disjunct air-pumping tas in which a hey the group t ted that loafing di t a socia ndivi 0 It may t de also predi that loafing would occu with a cap Participant were unable to inc their effort in "In both as they odin the colle onditio with a capable orker,individual partic ed by physical constraints rials than in the alo our inte es between the two c pensation hyp ere fre riding ing their orker to ca the ned three studies that varied diffe fel expecta 0 eacn experim ed on the two trials in which his desiened to be iewed as eaningful.In E 0 co-work nseenofninetrialswhentheirco-workerConsistcnl of co orker effort were manipulated.In Experim t 3.botl e for th of trial minori I that workin on a task the thev considered m ut of the d to poorly and would actually work age ot will a con harder collectively than coactively cluded a ian in especially con Experiment Participants pretested on interpersonal trust were asked to prevented it). work on an idea generation task either coactively or collectively ctations on and Harkins(1985) reported tha ants “matched eve ing that tain Thus. s of how tior nt in both c rking togeth d the or reduce y Io xnected thei to try hard or not to ury hardon me as potentia a shouting task.Jackson and Harkins demonstrated that the ithr participant

572 KIPLING D. WILLIAMS AND STEVEN J. KARAU coactively than when they work collectively because collectively they only share a diffused evaluation. On collective tasks, indi￾viduals are provided with an opportunity to obtain a positive evaluation without working hard themselves when they expect their co-workers to perform adequately. Indeed, studies that have measured participants' perceptions of their co-workers' performance indicate that they do not expect their co-workers to loaf (e.g., Harkins, Latane, & Williams, 1980; Jackson & Har￾kins, 1985; Latane, Williams, & Harkins, 1979; Williams et al., 1981). Therefore, both perspectives suggest that participants will demonstrate social compensation only if there is a strong reason for them to believe the group will do poorly and only if they care about the evaluation. Only a few studies have directly examined the effects of ex￾pectations of co-worker performance on individuals' effort in collective tasks. Moreover, studies that have manipulated ex￾pectations of co-worker performance are in disagreement, both with each other and with our predictions. Kerr (1983) studied individual effort on a disjunctive air-pumping task in which at least one member in the group had to reach an explicit criterion level for the group to succeed.2 He suggested that loafing would occur with a co-worker who is expected to perform well because individuals would view it as an opportunity to "free ride." He also predicted that loafing would occur with a capable co￾worker who is not expected to perform well because individuals would not want to be taken for a "sucker." In both collective conditions with a capable co-worker, individual participants reached the criterion fewer times (on average, seven of nine trials) than in the alone control condition (eight of nine trials). Although there were no differences between the two collective conditions, Kerr reasoned that in the success condition, partici￾pants were free riding, allowing their co-worker to carry the load, whereas in the failure condition, participants felt ex￾ploited and refused to play the role of the sucker. Although Kerr (1983) focused on the two trials in which his collective participants failed to reach criterion, we prefer to focus on the fact that participants, and hence their groups, suc￾ceeded on seven of nine trials when their co-worker consistently failed. We feel that participants actually compensated by being responsible for their group's success on the majority of trials and communicated their desire not to be a sucker on a minority of the trials. Essentially, Kerr's participants were able to come out of the experiment feeling successful while sending a mes￾sage to their co-worker that they were not willing to be a com￾plete sucker on all of the trials. We suggest that had Kerr in￾cluded a condition in which participants were especially con￾cerned about their group's performance, a larger compensation effect would have been found (if a ceiling effect would not have prevented it). In another study that manipulated expectations of co-worker performance on a collective task (i.e., shouting out loud), Jack￾son and Harkins (1985) reported that participants "matched" the level of output that they expected from their co-workers, suggesting that people seek to maintain equity in effort when working together. Participants' expectations of how hard their co-worker would work were manipulated so that participants expected their co-worker either to try hard or not to try hard on a shouting task. Jackson and Harkins demonstrated that the social loafing effect was eliminated and that participants matched their expectations of their co-worker's efforts. However, in Jackson and Harkins's (1985) experiment, the confederate/co-worker's statement of her intended effort was confounded with her evaluation of the experiment's worth. In the high effort condition, the co-worker told participants that she thought the experiment was interesting, and that she was going to try hard. In the low effort condition, she told partici￾pants that she thought the experiment was boring, and that she was not going to try hard. Hackman and Morris (1975) sug￾gested that interacting with group members who dislike the task will transmit that attitude to co-workers and cause them to minimize their efforts. Indeed, research on job attitudes (Salan￾cik & Pfeffer, 1978) suggests that workers' perceptions of and motivations toward their task are highly influenced by co￾workers' assessments of the task. Therefore, the confederate's assessment of the worth of the task could have alone accounted for why Jackson and Harkins did not find compensation in the low effort condition and why they found a main effect for partner effort. In the high effort (and high meaningfulness) con￾dition, our theoretical analysis would lead us to expect a social loafing effect. It may be that no loafing effect occurred because perceiving the task as especially meaningful resulted in a ceiling effect: Participants were unable to increase their effort in the coactive condition because they were already shouting as loudly as they could in the collective condition. Unlike some additive tasks, shouting is limited by physical constraints. Therefore, although these two experiments are relevant to our interests, they fail to provide a clear test of the social com￾pensation hypothesis. To examine the effects of expectations of co-worker performance on individual effort in a collective task, we designed three studies that varied different aspects of these expectations. In each experiment, participants worked either coactively or collectively on an idea generation task that was designed to be viewed as meaningful. In Experiment 1, expecta￾tions of co-worker performance were inferred from partici￾pants' interpersonal trust levels. In Experiment 2, expectations of co-worker effort were manipulated. In Experiment 3, both expectations of co-worker ability and of the meaningfulness of the task were manipulated. In all three studies, we hypothe￾sized that, when working on a task that they considered mean￾ingful, participants would compensate for a co-worker whom they expected to perform poorly and would actually work harder collectively than coactively. Experiment 1 Participants pretested on interpersonal trust were asked to work on an idea generation task either coactively or collectively. 2 There are a number of differences between Kerr's (1983) paradigm and the more typical social loafing paradigm. His participants were completely identifiable to themselves, their co-worker, and the experi￾menter. Thus, evaluation was strong and present in both coactive and collective settings, leaving responsibility for and control over the out￾come as potential causes for reduced effort. Additionally, Kerr used a disjunctive task that had a threshold criterion for success such that if either participant succeeded, the group succeeded

SOCIAL LOAFING AND SOCIAL COMPENSATION 573 Wee would carry ther the group task.Participants cause th would tend to expect others to loaf and on to writ harder so as to maintainr e uses Method and de iocenterreente ded out a rietquestioanai plit-hal s of 68 and5 on tw eted the question and dismissed. Result Performance measures.Overall,there was a significant loaf ing more when wor (high tru 120)=5.51,p<.03.More m the esults ort the cdtoptrto The the task nt to the pa Table1).A prio contrasts rev led tha pro t in the time prov ded.Inou and 21 (39) (de Ccondtionl co of 4 to 8w greeted by the expe e led to a re iddle of the chairs w 20)=4.03,p< .03,in that medi other.On ped pen uld b s that varied a ad th along Participants in eareinterese meani chec hty (0 e f an ct an tas be to 49 tifa ipants was not significant (20).Debrief and su ses you c tant t as mar eve th An other question asked participar 00-in nted the Supporting the notion that low trusters expected their co- ing: 'lnibothEpcrimeatland3,thereappearedtobeomehct me log tra uses as your partners I want to sce how many useach iormationrcvealedidenticalpaternsofmeansandsignih cant

SOCIAL LOAFING AND SOCIAL COMPENSATION 573 We expected participants scoring high or medium on trust to loaf collectively, because they would tend to expect that others would carry their own weight on the group task. Participants scoring low in trust were hypothesized to compensate collec￾tively because they would tend to expect others to loaf and would feel compelled to work harder so as to maintain a favor￾able evaluation of themselves. Method Participants and design. In a pretest session at the beginning of the quarter at the University of Washington, 1,085 introductory psy￾chology students responded to Rotter's (1967) Interpersonal Trust Scale. Rotter reported test-retest coefficients of .68 and .56 on two samples, p .20). Debriefing and sub￾sequent research, however, has led us to believe that partici￾pants had difficulty interpreting this particular question. An￾other question asked participants if they thought the others in their session generated fewer, the same, or more uses than they did (on a 100-point scale in which 50 represented the same). Supporting the notion that low trusters expected their co- 3 In both Experiments 1 and 3, there appeared to be some heterosce￾dasticity in the number of uses data. Reanalyses of these data sets using a log transformation revealed identical patterns of means and signifi￾cant effects

574 KIPLING D.WILLIAMS AND STEVEN J.KARAL Tablel 0ygGigaRaoraKaeasaAnaono eheo Work condition performance on the task was thought to be related to intell Trust level(quintile) Coactive Collective thatsocialoainewoulloccrorparicipants 袋 Method s at pur SD r participa vorkers to reduce their efforts when workine collectively the only condition in ration task used in experiment which pa icipants though that their co Experime 2,wi地 to 49.1).Wh vere other asked hov ndthowninito in ants fr的 On each desk sa pair of headphones xt to them the floor.In th working coactively and working colectivel Discussion The behavioral data from exp ortfor the hypothesis that levels oft that is thou ors de eorcliminatcsocialloainthisndinedg The exper menter then told the participants that a recent theor artic strate a causal link between factors,it could be that other var th hceinsimia ealso told that once their uses had been counted.the uses rust ated b orker I are i mance Experiment 2 rork The purpose of Experiment 2 was to dir er's efforts.Rather have the work effort as an evaluation of the task's meaningfulness

574 KIPLING D. WILLIAMS AND STEVEN J. KARAU Table 1 Mean Number of Uses Generated for a Knife as a Function of Levels of Trust and Work Condition Work condition Trust level (quintile) Coactive Collective Low M n SD Medium M n SD High M n SD 24.45 20 7.73 25.48 21 9.19 33.20 20 10.04 29.55 22 9.05 21.05 20 3.71 22.61 23 5.86 workers to reduce their efforts when working collectively, the only condition in which participants thought that their co￾workers generated fewer uses than they did (M= 42.91) was the low trust, collective condition (95% confidence interval = 36.7 to 49.1). When asked how much effort participants thought people generally exert on collective tasks compared with indi￾vidual tasks, they indicated (as has often been the case in pre￾vious social loafing research) that there would not be any differ￾ence (M = 50.8, in which 50 represented the same) between working coactively and working collectively. Discussion The behavioral data from Experiment 1 provide strong sup￾port for the hypothesis that levels of trust determine whether social loafing or social compensation will occur. Whereas other research in social loafing has shown that various factors de￾crease or eliminate social loafing, this finding demonstrates a significant increase in effort by individuals on a collective task. Social loafing once again has been demonstrated for the me￾dium trusting person (assuming this typifies the average partici￾pant in past research), and even more so for the high trusting person. Low trusters, however, were actually more productive when working collectively than when working coactively. Of course, as with any study using individual differences to demon￾strate a causal link between factors, it could be that other vari￾ables associated with trust accounted for such a pattern. Fur￾thermore, we did not assess directly our assumption that low trusters compensated because they thought their co-workers were loafing. Although the results from Experiment 1 are in￾triguing and suggestive, we felt that it was necessary to submit the social compensation hypothesis to more direct tests by ma￾nipulating experimentally expectations of co-worker perfor￾mance. Experiment 2 The purpose of Experiment 2 was to directly manipulate the participants' expectations of their co-worker's efforts. Rather than link the co-worker's intended effort with his or her ap￾praisal of the task, as did Jackson and Harkins (1985), we kept the appraisal constant and positive throughout. To ensure that the task was perceived as meaningful, we told participants that performance on the task was thought to be related to intelli￾gence. Two levels of co-worker effort (high or low) were crossed with two work conditions (coactive or collective). We predicted that social loafing would occur for participants working with a high effort co-worker, whereas social compensation would oc￾cur for those working with a low effort co-worker. Method Design and participants. The experiment used a 2 (low vs. high co-worker effort) X 2 (coactive vs. collective work condition) between￾subjects factorial design. Participants were 49 introductory psychology students at Purdue University who fulfilled partial course credit by their participation. Ten participants were eliminated from the final analyses because they indicated possible suspicions regarding loafing (they had read about it in the textbook), the authenticity of the confeder￾ate, or the experimental hypotheses.4 This resulted in a total of 39 participants. Procedure. The idea generation task used in Experiment 1 was used in Experiment 2, with slight modifications. Two participants (one of whom was a male confederate) were greeted by the male experimenter and shown into the laboratory. They were seated next to each other and were separated by a large filing cabinet that prevented the two partici￾pants from seeing one another. On each desk was a pair of headphones, a pen, and a box of blank slips of paper. In the coactive condition, participants had separate boxes placed next to them on the floor. In the collective condition, a common box (with an opening in the top accessi￾ble to both participants but preventing each from seeing inside) was placed between the two tables. The experimenter then read similar instructions as were read in Experiment 1. In addition, participants were told: \bu both will be working on a "brainstorming-type" task. The purpose of this experiment is to examine rapid thinking, a quality that is thought to be highly correlated with intelligence in adults. Your task is to come up with as many uses for an object as you can. \bu will be working side by side, without communicating. The experimenter then told the participants that a recent theory suggested that rapid thinking was highly correlated with intelligence, and that it was therefore extremely important that they come up with as many uses as they possibly could. The experimenter also told partici￾pants that he would count up their individual scores (or group scores, if in the collective condition) at the end of the session and tell them how many uses they had produced. In addition, their scores would be com￾pared with those of other participants who had been in similar re￾search studies at other universities. Participants in the collective condi￾tion were also told that once their uses had been counted, the uses 4 The distribution of suspicious participants was approximately even across three of the four conditions, with no suspicious partici￾pants in the high effort, coactive condition. The suspicious partici￾pants in the high effort, collective condition tended to believe the co￾worker's announcement to be sarcastic, thereby interpreting the mes￾sage to be the opposite, as was intended. In the low effort conditions, suspicious participants found it difficult to believe that their co-worker would not work hard on a task they found interesting. We felt it was necessary to have the co-worker state that the task was interesting so participants would not interpret the co-worker's statement of intended effort as an evaluation of the task's meaningfulness

SOCIAL LOAFING AND SOCIAL COMPENSATION 575 0001.Pu and left the rimenter would be at least some ewhat likely to know thei tha ncndedetot d that the experimenter might be able to make a rough ork ve hard .In the ork hare "Th of ir ar ation of th man of the task whic The main dependn arablc of not pr the te'sa n-participants a effort and t work condition in the ntia anceex ection,Fl,35) 6,p9 own h ab 2.a pr h t effort co- tive=31.3 and collective o the d The nter the read 017 .03,whereas participants w ng w n a g th actively (M.5).(1) 1.16.D ce.Then.the an e in cluded to provide background information regarding partic left the to star The tape then pla ved 12 min of new gh to pre g to rned with how wel her i Then the articipan they p d on the task(M=67.8).There were no signifi ofuses w ted The relevant to partic Results ants th ught they had n-poinscale high tive/collective work co nditio ariable Participa he y came up with more uses than did col had e M al ed h rd th manipulated:Participants in the high on though There was a significant difference in self- ported hat t (M .001.Participants were als asked hov w much abilit 002,whe reas there were no differ en wo ney thoug had at th est that nartici. nigher ability to high effo ers (M=66.6)than to lov 0,35296 58.5 hard hy 0 sked whe ther the experim nter Discussion ntebatcxtentth thought the ld he able t had perfor ually More partic that the nter wa nteres ted ir lual perlorm .84,P< the ex menter would be more likely to be able to tell how e.h the n 412). F0

SOCIAL LOAFING AND SOCIAL COMPENSATION 575 would be discarded, and that this would prevent the experimenter from knowing any person's individual score. At this point, the experimenter "noticed" that he had "forgotten" to bring his stopwatch and left the room to retrieve it. While the experi￾menter was out of the room, the confederate reported on his level of intended effort by saying one of two things. In the low effort condition, the confederate said, "This sounds like an interesting experiment, but I don't think I'm going to work very hard." In the high effort condition, the confederate said, "This sounds like an interesting experiment; I think I'm really going to work hard." The confederate's comment was worded this way so that co-worker effort could be manipulated without varying the level of evaluation of the meaningfulness of the task (which remained constant and positive). Furthermore, because the experi￾menter was not present during the confederate's announcement of in￾tended effort, there was no basis from which the participant could infer differential performance expectations on the part of the experimenter. After a brief pause long enough to retrieve a stopwatch but short enough to prevent any conversation between the participants except for the confederate's comment, the experimenter reentered the room with the stopwatch in his hand. The experimenter then read some final instructions indicating that the participants would be listening to mu￾sic during the task (this was done to mask the sound emitted from writing the uses down on the slips of paper so that participants could not monitor one another's performance). Then, after the participants put their headphones on, the experimenter started the tape, started the stopwatch, and left the room. Instructions on the tape informed the participants of what object they were to think of uses for (a knife) and when to start. The tape then played 12 min of new age music at a volume loud enough to prevent the participant from talking to the confederate and from monitoring the confederate's writing. Instruc￾tions on the tape then told the participants to stop writing and wait for further instructions. Then the participants filled out a questionnaire and were debriefed and dismissed, after which the number of uses was counted. Results Manipulation checks. Four questions (using 100-point scales) were included as manipulation checks for the effort and coac￾tive/collective work condition variables. Participants were asked how hard they thought their co-worker had tried on the task. Differential levels of co-worker effort were successfully manipulated: Participants in the high effort condition thought that their co-workers had tried harder (M = 76.6) than did par￾ticipants in the low effort condition (M = 50.7), F(l, 35) = 20.35, p < .001. Participants were also asked how much ability they thought their co-worker had at the type of task they had just completed. Although participants attributed somewhat higher ability to high effort co-workers (M = 66.6) than to low effort co-workers (M = 58.5), this difference was not significant, F(l,35) = 2.96, p=.Q9. Participants were also asked whether the experimenter was interested in their individual or their group's performance, and to what extent they thought the experimenter would be able to tell how well they had performed individually. More partici￾pants in the coactive condition (68%) than in the collective con￾dition (0%) reported that the experimenter was interested in their individual performance, x 2 (l, N = 39) = 18.84, p < .0001.5 Similarly, participants in the coactive condition thought that the experimenter would be more likely to be able to tell how they had performed individually (M = 11 A) than participants in the collective condition (M = 41.2), F(l, 35) = 35.05, p < .0001. Furthermore, brief discussions with those few partici￾pants in the collective condition who reported thinking that the experimenter would be at least somewhat likely to know their individual performance revealed that these participants did not believe that their actual scores would be known, but thought instead that the experimenter might be able to make a rough estimate as to their performance on the basis of the total group score. So it appears that both the effort and work conditions were manipulated successfully. Performance measures. The main dependent variable of in￾terest was the number of uses for a knife that the participants generated. A 2 X 2 between-participants analysis of variance was performed on the data. There was a significant interaction between co-worker effort and work condition in the predicted direction, F(\, 35) = 5.16, p < .03. As shown in Table 2, a priori contrasts revealed that participants loafed in the presence of a high effort co-worker (coactive M = 31.3 and collective M = 22.7), «(17) = 2.06, p < .03, whereas participants working with a low effort co-worker were somewhat more productive collec￾tively (M =29.2) than coactively (M = 24.5), t(l8) = -1.16, p < .14. There were no significant effects for gender. Ancillary data. A number of questionnaire items were in￾cluded to provide background information regarding partici￾pants' perceptions of the experimental task. Participants seemed to find the task rather meaningful (M = 72.2; the scales ranged from 1 [not at all] to 100 [very]), moderately fun (M = 57.2), and not very difficult to perform (M = 21.4). Participants also reported that they were indeed concerned with how well they performed on the task (M= 67.8). There were no signifi￾cant differences across variables. There were several questionnaire measures relevant to partic￾ipants' perceptions of their own effort. In the low effort condi￾tions, there were no differences between the number of uses the coactive or collective participants thought they had generated (coactive M = 26.7 and collective M = 26). In the high effort conditions, however, coactive participants tended to report that they came up with more uses than did collective participants (coactive M=26.3 and collective M= 19.9), t(l 7) = 2.1, p < .03. Participants were also asked how hard they had tried on the task. There was a significant difference in self-reported effort between work conditions in the high co-worker effort condition (coactive M = 90.6 and collective M = 71.1), /(17) = 3.5, p < .002, whereas there were no differences between work condi￾tions in the low co-worker effort condition (coactive M = 77.9 and collective M = 76.4), ns. These data suggest that partici￾pants may have been attentive to how hard they were working on the task. Discussion These results mildly support the social compensation hy￾pothesis and stand in contrast to the matching hypothesis. Par- 5 In debriefing, it became clear that many of the participants in the coactive condition misunderstood the intent of this question, thinking that even though the experimenter was looking at each individual's performance, he was still interested in how both of them did. This seems to account for why as many as 32% of the coactive participants thought the experimenter was interested in the group performance

576 KIPLING D WILLIAMS AND STEVEN J KARAU Table2 lack of motivation.We predicted that social compensation Work condition Coa tive Collective 2445 29.20 s.then they would not have bothered con pensating 898 958 thei ad ha chose 沿0 22.61 ingfulness.In a broader context,mea aningfulness might be d 919 ine ance to the 7.99 in terms of importance or sign u ch par who ended towork har on the task.In the high meaningfulness condition,the expc (a ce.D pertorming co-work a positive the 1 ented as rather useless and trivial.We expected that pa Is in they would loaf in all other conditions. differences in effort a and coulecu e co Method k they h The high their own effort,possibly for strategi reasons.When they ex lness)b een-subjects fa on.Eight p ry strong in this sation stra y is ade ted when participants work ithalowef arise from a variety nd task used in Experi produce e demons tion,the exprimenter ergn ho acted nterpe expectations similarly could be as being silly and Int让 high ingful ess conditi erred fr east reduced arch by Kerr(983)has sh that participants loafed les ple o air-pumpin but they stil did not sua th Simila rly,a study byT they as low e given there was still reduced for ability nditions part heirciornwhcnwotikingoleg be evaluated coact Experiment 3 e student exn htmcaninlncoYing We conducte a hat the rity of participant toexperiment

576 KIPLING D. WILLIAMS AND STEVEN J. KARAU Table 2 Mean Number of Uses Generated for a Knife as a Function of Perceived Co-Worker Effort and Work Condition Work condition Perceived co-worker effort Coactive Collective Low M n SD High M n SD 24.45 10 8.98 31.30 12 9.19 29.20 10 9.58 22.61 7 7.99 ticipants loafed with a co-worker who intended to work hard and tended to compensate for one who did not. It appears that the participants were willing to carry the burden of a poorly performing co-worker to maintain a positive evaluation for the group and, therefore, for themselves. Interestingly, participants seemed to be aware of the amount of effort they were exerting on the task. This is in contrast to prior social loafing research that typically has reported no per￾ceived differences in effort across coactive and collective condi￾tions. We suggest that when individuals are aware of how hard their co-workers intend to work, they become more attentive to their own effort, possibly for strategic reasons. When they ex￾pect their co-workers to work hard, they can make a conscious decision to rely on the efforts of others. Unfortunately, because our social compensation effect was not very strong in this ex￾periment, it is difficult to determine whether a similarly conscious compensation strategy is adopted when participants must work with a low effort co-worker. We suspect that expectations about co-worker behavior that produce social compensation may arise from a variety of sources. We have demonstrated their occurrence for expecta￾tions based on self-reported co-worker effort and on levels of interpersonal trust. These expectations similarly could be based on co-worker ability or on other attributions made re￾garding the co-worker (either internal to the participant or in￾ferred from statements made by others). Indeed, there is some support for social compensation, or at least reduced levels of loafing, based on low co-worker ability. Research by Kerr (1983) has shown that participants loafed less with incapable, rather than capable, co-workers on the disjunc￾tive air-pumping task, but they still did not surpass their coac￾tive performance levels. Similarly, a study by Thompson, Jack￾son, Williams, and Latane (1980) has shown that, although there was still reduced effort across ability conditions, partici￾pants were less likely to reduce their effort when working collec￾tively with a low ability partner. Experiment 3 We conducted Experiment 3 to examine two major issues. One goal was to determine whether social compensation would generalize to circumstances in which the co-worker is not ex￾pected to perform well because of lack of ability rather than lack of motivation. We predicted that social compensation would also occur when poor co-worker performance was ex￾pected on the basis of low ability. A second purpose was to test the hypothesis that social compensation is more likely to occur when the participant is concerned about the evaluation of the group's performance. In the first two experiments, we speculated that had partici￾pants perceived their participation in the task as worthless or meaningless, then they would not have bothered compensating for their poorly performing co-workers but would have chosen to loaf. In Experiment 3, we directly manipulated task mean￾ingfulness. In a broader context, meaningfulness might be de￾fined in terms of importance or significance to the individual, to others, to scientific progress, or to life in general. Specific to our experiment, we operationally defined meaningfulness by invoking manipulations that affected the degree to which partic￾ipants were concerned with the evaluation of their performance on the task.6 In the high meaningfulness condition, the experi￾ment was presented as important and related to intelligence. In the low meaningfulness condition, the experiment was pre￾sented as rather useless and trivial. We expected that partici￾pants would only be willing to compensate for a low ability co-worker in the high task meaningfulness condition, whereas they would loaf in all other conditions. Method Design and participants. The experiment used a 2 (low vs. high co-worker ability) X 2 (coactive vs. collective work condition) X 2 (low vs. high task meaningfulness) between-subjects factorial design. Partic￾ipants were 88 introductory psychology students at Purdue University who fulfilled partial course credit by their participation. Eight partici￾pants were eliminated from the final analyses because they either knew the experimenter or confederate personally or they expressed, during debriefing, knowledge about social loafing, suspicion about the experi￾mental hypotheses, or both. Procedure. The same general paradigm and task used in Experi￾ment 2 were used in this experiment. Meaningfulness manipulation. In the low meaningfulness condi￾tion, the experimenter was a female undergraduate who acted very uninvolved and uninterested in the experiment and who presented the task as being silly and trivial. In the high meaningfulness condition, the experimenter was a male graduate student who acted very involved and interested in the experiment and who presented the task as being very meaningful and related to intelligence.7 6 Although our manipulation of task meaningfulness involved multi￾ple operations, it was necessary to make this manipulation as strong as possible to overcome participants' tendency to report that the task was at least moderately meaningful. Pretesting indicated that participants were very hesitant to view the task as low in meaningfulness unless they were given a clear basis for discounting the value of the experi￾ment. Despite the multiple operations used to manipulate meaningful￾ness, we still expected participants to loaf on the low meaningfulness task because their individual contributions could be evaluated coacti￾vely but not collectively. 7 It was our intention to manipulate task meaningfulness by using a graduate student experimenter for the high meaningful condition and an undergraduate experimenter for the low meaningful condition. The resulting differences in the gender of the experimenters were due to the availability of experimenters. An analysis of variance failed to reveal any significant effects due to participant gender, suggesting that the similarity of participant to experimenter did not influence effort

SOCIAL LOAFING AND SOCIAL COMPENSATION 577 b able to tell how they had performed indi dually (M- ou prief disc hf e few participants in the collective ondition who reported be at le Ied that the come up with as m uses for they could and that thev participants did not believe that their actual b tiCipamsgreheneiherpelieyhandedbiehmcaningoinesg able to that the exp basis of gr oup score Partic orte lly, ores would be In addit on,there was a 03.In the high meaninefulness condition.p oiRa the ce their es had heen co uld he prevent the expe nowing iker (M nts evalu the dition the ss task as e ing m task. the both more 432. 72 an meaningfulness task.They arder (M 74.3F,72 ions on the part of the experimente s=75.8vs.66.1,F0,71)=6.50,p<.02,on the high Results ingfuness task than on the low mea cr t was s the number of use that the pa ants came up A n-part olective work condition,and task meaningfuess variables C,2=5.10.p 03.Partici nts ge rated mo support for the success of king coactively(M 31.0 tha Participants n the abiliy fulness i ction.F.72-=10.46.p<.005.Partic nts in the ne sked to the high dition orked harder col ed high abilityc 28.9) diti Co-Worker k me ful 63 (M 12.3 71 332 ars that partic ants e ted both than did collective participants (M=21.9).In the low co o-workers,and he lity More participants in the coactive condition(7)than in the ionM=19.3),08) 3.01.D .005 Similarly.in the high

SOCIAL LOAFING AND SOCIAL COMPENSATION 577 Once the participants were seated, the experimenter read some intro￾ductory instructions. In the low meaningfulness condition, the experi￾menter read: "I got an incomplete in one of my classes, so now I have to run this study. It's about everyday objects, or something like that. Here's your instructions." In the high meaningfulness condition, the experi￾menter read: "'You both will be working on a 'brainstorming-type' task. The purpose of this experiment is to examine rapid thinking, a quality that is thought to be highly correlated with intelligence in adults." Participants in both conditions were told that their task was to come up with as many uses for an object as they could and that they would be working side by side without communicating. Participants were then either politely handed (high meaningfulness) or thrown (low meaningfulness) instruction booklets that varied ac￾cording to the work condition. Although both booklets were clearly legible, booklets in the high meaningfulness condition were nicely printed laser copies, whereas booklets in the low meaningfulness con￾dition were homely looking typewritten copies. Finally, participants in the high meaningfulness condition were told that their scores would be compared with those of other participants who had been in similar research studies at other universities. In both work conditions, the experimenter told participants that he or she would count up their individual or group scores at the end of the session and tell them how many uses they had produced. Participants in the collective condition were also told that once their uses had been counted, the uses would be discarded, and that this would prevent the experimenter from knowing any person's individual score. Ability manipulation. At the point at which the experimenter "no￾ticed" that he or she had "forgotten" to bring a stopwatch and left the room to retrieve one, the confederate reported on his level of ability by saying one of two things. In the low co-worker ability condition, the confederate said, "I'm not very good at this sort of thing; I can never come up with any ideas." In the high co-worker ability condition, the confederate said, "I'm really good at this sort of thing; I can usually come up with a lot of ideas." Because the experimenter was not present during the confederate's statement of ability, there was no basis from which the participant could infer differential performance expecta￾tions on the part of the experimenter. Results Manipulation checks. Five questions (using 100-point scales) were included as manipulation checks for the ability, coactive/ collective work condition, and task meaningfulness variables. Several other questions also provided support for the success of the manipulations. Participants in the high co-worker ability condition reported that their co-workers had more ability (M = 70.3) than did par￾ticipants in the low co-worker ability condition (M = 60.0), F(l, 71) = 10.42, p < .005. Similarly, when participants were asked to guess how many uses their co-worker had come up with, they reported that they expected high ability co-workers to have gen￾erated more uses (M = 30.9) than low ability co-workers (M = 23.4), F(1,72) = 6.84, p < .02. Participants were also asked how hard they thought their co-worker had tried on the task. Partici￾pants attributed higher effort to high ability co-workers (M = 76.3) than to low ability co-workers (M= 65.6), F(\, 11) = 7.58, p < .01. Therefore, it appears that participants expected both high ability and high effort from high ability co-workers, and relatively less ability and less effort from low ability co-workers. More participants in the coactive condition (70%) than in the collective condition (8%) reported that the experimenter was interested in their individual performance, x 2 (l. N = 80) = 32.92, p < .0001. Similarly, participants in the coactive condi￾tion thought that the experimenter would be more likely to be able to tell how they had performed individually (M = 74.8) than did participants in the collective condition (Af= 44.1), F(i, 78) = 28.43, p < .0001. Furthermore, brief discussions with those few participants in the collective condition who reported thinking that the experimenter would be at least somewhat likely to know their individual performance revealed that these participants did not believe that their actual scores would be known, but thought instead that the experimenter might be able to make a rough estimate as to their performance on the basis of the total group score. Participants in the high meaningfulness condition reported finding the task more meaningful (M= 70.6) than participants in the low meaningfulness condition (M = 59.5), F(l, 72) = 4.78, p < .04. In addition, there was a significant Co-Worker Ability X Task Meaningfulness interaction, F(l, 72) = 5.04, p < .03. In the high meaningfulness condition, participants rated the task as being meaningful regardless of their co-worker's ability (low ability M = 69.9 and high ability M = 71.3), F(l, 36) < 1; however, in the low meaningfulness condition, partici￾pants rated the task as being less meaningful when they worked with a low ability co-worker (M= 47.5) than with a high ability co-worker (M = 71.6), F(l, 36) = 9.36, p < .005. Four other questions provide support for the claim that participants evalu￾ated the high meaningfulness task as being more important and meaningful than the low meaningfulness task. Specifically, they reported that the high meaningfulness task was both more difficult (Ms = 30.3 vs. 14.35), F(\, 72) = 13.83, p < .001, and more fun (Ms = 61.1 vs. 43.2), F(l, 72)= 10.20, p<. 005, than the low meaningfulness task. They also reported that they worked harder (Ms = 84.5 vs. 74.3), F(l, 72) = 8.99, p < .005, and that they guessed that their co-worker had worked harder (Ms = 75.8 vs. 66.1), F(l, 71) = 6.50, p < .02, on the high meaningfulness task than on the low meaningfulness task. Performance measures. The main dependent variable of in￾terest was the number of uses that the participants came up with. A 2 X 2 X 2 between-participants analysis of variance was performed on the data. Overall, there was a significant loafing effect, F(l, 72) = 5.10, p < .03. Participants generated more uses when working coactively (M = 31.0) than collectively (M = 26.4). There was also a significant Work Condition X Task Meaningfulness interaction, F(l, 72) = 10.46, p < .005. Partici￾pants in the low meaningfulness condition worked harder coac￾tively (M= 33.2) than collectively (M= 21.9), whereas partici￾pants in the high meaningfulness condition worked harder col￾lectively (M = 30.9) than coactively (M= 28.9). Most important, the predicted three-way Work Condition X Co-Worker Ability X Task Meaningfulness interaction was sig￾nificant, F(l, 72) = 7.81, p < .01. Within low task meaningful￾ness, a significant loafing effect was found, F(l, 36) = 12.39, p < .005. Coactive participants came up with more uses (M= 33.2) than did collective participants (M = 21.9). In the low co￾worker ability condition, participants produced more uses in the coactive condition (M = 34.9) than in the collective condi￾tion (M = 19.3), /(18) = 3.01, p < .005. Similarly, in the high ability condition, participants produced more uses in the coac￾tive condition (M = 31.4) than in the collective condition (M =

578 KIPLING D.WILLIAMS AND STEVEN J KARAU Table3 and Work Condition their co- mber of uses parti Work condition Coactive Collective more uses than theirc Low meaningfulness task uses.95%confidence interval =2.16 to 9.64).There was a nt three-way intera 34.90 14.10 8.33 the uses data.F(l,72).43,07.Examining the simpl etw and colle 6 ne condition (coa 78.4and 0 is last finding High meaningfulness task creased co ective effor 沿0 5.91 891 Discussion 0 得0 The results of Experiment3 lend strong support to the social "s0 850 8.74 ation cffect gen hich th cial comper 24.5),t(18)=1.84,p<.05 (see Table 3).There were no signifi- the social compensation effect was limited to relatively me teraction bew rke ability and work condition in the PrdicteddirectionnF in the 7.67,p witha lowabil ay co Participant eported trying the a, nthy more uses than their co. ntrast to 受m ively (M 32.4)than coactively (=23.3) dsignifca E supported. were aware of their social loafing. General Discussion o-workers in the high mean rectly sured th ugh an inter nal trust scale sub veen and -.24,ns) ted their n to tho ems reported with the questionnaire measures fing.We do not necessarily mean by this term that they are a signi ingor aiding ther er,bu that they ee estim ctable group product that will result in a positive evalua than collectively.).Fd.36)5.63.03.wh 9. the task sary that the pa pants in the high mance on the t sk)as imp tant or meaningful Apparently if Iness cor they had come up participants viewed the task as meaningless,it was easier for

578 KIPLING D. WILLIAMS AND STEVEN J. KARAU Table 3 Mean Number of Uses Generated for a Knife as a Function of Perceived Co-Worker Ability, Task Meaningfulness, and Work Condition Work condition Perceived co-worker ability Coactive Collective Low M n SD High M n SD Low meaningfulness task 34.90 10 14.10 31.40 10 8.47 19.30 10 8.33 24.50 10 8.28 Low M n SD High M n SD High meaningfulness task 23.30 10 5.91 34.40 10 8.50 32.40 10 8.91 29.30 10 8.74 24.5), £(18) = 1.84, p <.!!. When working with a low ability co-worker, however, the opposite effect was found: Participants compensated, work￾ing harder collectively (M = 32.4) than coactively (M = 23.3), t({ 8) = 2.69, p < .01. Therefore, the major experimental hypoth￾eses concerning social loafing and social compensation were supported. In addition, we created an index of relative co-worker ability (co-worker ability minus own ability) to measure the degree to which participants thought that their co-workers were better at the task than they were. Consistent with our predictions, only in the high meaningfulness, collective condition were feelings of inferior co-worker ability associated with increases in partici￾pants' effort, r(20) = -.44, p < .05 (other rs ranged between -.11 and —.24, ns). Ancillary data. In addition to those items reported with the manipulation checks just described, there were a number of questionnaire measures relevant to the task meaningfulness and social compensation hypotheses. There was a significant Work Condition X Task Meaningfulness interaction such that participants in the low meaningfulness condition estimated that they had come up with more uses coactively (M = 29.3) than collectively (M= 21.3), F(\, 36) = 5.63, p < .03, whereas there was a nonsignificant tendency for participants in the high meaningfulness condition to estimate that they had come up with more uses collectively (M = 29.8) than coactively (M = 23.6), F(l, 36) = 1.9. As an index of perceived relative contribu￾tions, we subtracted the number of uses participants thought their co-workers generated from the number of uses partici￾pants thought they had generated themselves. The only condi￾tion in which participants reported contributing significantly more uses than their co-workers was in the high meaningful￾ness, low co-worker ability, collective condition (M= 5.9 more uses, 95% confidence interval = 2.16 to 9.64). There was a marginally significant three-way interaction for self-reported effort that produced a pattern of means similar to that found for the uses data, F(l, 72) = 3.43, p < .07. Examining the simple contrasts between the coactive and collective conditions, the only significant difference was found in the high meaningful￾ness, low co-worker ability condition (coactive M= 78.4 and collective M= 92.9), t(l&) = -2.81, p < .01. This last finding suggests that participants who worked with low ability co￾workers on a high meaningfulness task were aware of their in￾creased collective effort. Discussion The results of Experiment 3 lend strong support to the social compensation hypotheses. The social compensation effect gen￾eralized to a situation in which the participants' co-worker was unable, rather than unwilling, to contribute. Apparently, attri￾butions regarding the reasons for poor performance did not alter the social compensation response. Moreover, as expected, the social compensation effect was limited to relatively mean￾ingful tasks: If participants were not convinced of the signifi￾cance of the task, then they chose to socially loaf regardless of their co-worker's ability. It is interesting to note that participants in the high meaning￾fulness, low co-worker ability condition seemed to be aware of their social compensation. Participants reported trying the hardest in this condition and also thought that they had gener￾ated significantly more uses than their co-worker. In contrast to Experiment 2, but consistent with prior research, participants in the remaining collective conditions did not report that they were aware of their social loafing. General Discussion Taken together, the results of the present studies suggest that expectations of co-worker performance, either manipulated di￾rectly or measured through an interpersonal trust scale, sub￾stantially affect whether or not social loafing will occur. When participants expected their co-workers to be unreliable (Experi￾ment 1) or unwilling (Experiment 2), or unable (Experiment 3), they exhibited social compensation, an effect opposite to social loafing. We do not necessarily mean by this term that they are helping or aiding their co-worker, but rather that they feel com￾pelled to compensate for their weaker co-worker to attain a respectable group product that will result in a positive evalua￾tion for themselves. Furthermore, it was necessary that the par￾ticipants viewed the task (or the evaluation of the group's perfor￾mance on the task) as important or meaningful. Apparently, if participants viewed the task as meaningless, it was easier for

SOCIAL LOAFING AND SOCIAL COMPENSATION 579 e ation will be respe Social Compensation,Social Loafing,and Social Facilitation Possible Motives for Social Compensation We are not suggesting that the individual is concerned only to qucstion eth ial lo ial fn cor ollect out be a小 cdcothecyariousocialaclafionprog 198 put rth ir tion。 aluation is the nby mear 0 -confict (Baron,1986)can explain the results because.in obtaining a diffused individual evaluation,and this concer ons,the sam umber of people were phys ally pre ent with thals and Darley's (1987)notion that use thei plain the al c tion ugh we ar sting thatso ri- rsonal trust,the particip ants'efforts in the g the uld lead to social co ation.One such motive in ugh the evaluati ofthe Parti t to protect their co nd th ve condition,a fir nding no ny social facilita ion th uld manipu ndeed. the is also contrary to the co-worker or the cok of th s to test th ng Th is not the first tin The second vork on and williams (1985)found that on a ng ety a ing fro om combining s leads to b cinant to en a d in thi On the ot hand.when participar ght ge in t ork lati tudies to show .This then. is also t take credit for it;if th ive product is oor,then the ms a favo rable self evalu s in coary t whe in which the par ant is lov in iden 's n ntradict nd nding of their own inputs.Attemp s could b ecqet son anc am (198)and Szyma dki and imenter)from c cern for se ause of reduced evaluation nay be p t to av likely that concern for the experimenter's evaluation was more people's contributions,the potenc of the positive sahent【o our participants than was the r the n is gre( ly din or the to the outputs tot ng th lly,th hard be g that under certain co potential for a poor ct ine,1990 go beyond hiso nd its n when dual et noDd,tcouldnot

SOCIAL LOAFING AND SOCIAL COMPENSATION 579 them to discount the task rather than to increase their effort when working with low ability co-workers. Social Compensation, Social Loafing, and Social Facilitation One interesting aspect of the social compensation effect is that, on the surface, performance appears to be "facilitated" when working in groups. It is natural, then, to question whether this finding could be better understood from a social facilita￾tion perspective. We do not believe the results of these studies are due to the various social facilitation processes put forth in the literature. Neither mere presence (Zajonc, 1980) nor distrac￾tion-conflict (Baron, 1986) can explain the results because, in all sessions, the same number of people were physically present at all sessions, providing the same amounts of arousal and dis￾traction. Evaluation apprehension (Cottrell, 1972) cannot ex￾plain the social compensation results in any simple manner, because regardless of their co-worker's efforts or their own level of interpersonal trust, the participants' efforts in the group per￾formance were equally unidentifiable and, hence, unevaluable. We have demonstrated a finding in which there is increased effort in a collective condition even though the evaluation of the participant is lower than in the coactive condition, a finding not predicted by any social facilitation theory. Indeed, the social compensation effect is also contrary to explanations given for social loafing. This is not the first time that participants have shown better performance collectively than coactively.8 Jackson and Williams (1985) found that on a complex task, less effort resulted in better performance. They proposed that the reduction in anxiety and evaluation appre￾hension resulting from combining efforts leads to better perfor￾mance when anxiety interferes with performance. The present social compensation effect, however, cannot be viewed in this light. The idea generation task used in the present experiments was a standard, relatively easy task that has been used in several studies to show the social loafing effect. This, then, is also the first demonstration of improved performance attributable to increased effort, as a result of working collectively. Contrary to social loafing, social compensation also occurs in collective tasks in which the participant is low in identifiability. Our explanation for the social compensation effect is not, however, contradictory to our understanding of social loafing. As Jackson and Williams (1985) and Szymanski and Harkins (1987) have suggested, working collectively is demotivating be￾cause of reduced evaluation. Presumably, people generally de￾sire a positive evaluation or want to avoid a negative evaluation. When their contribution to the product is pooled with other people's contributions, the potency of the positive or negative evaluation is greatly diminished. If people's contributions were made noticeable (even to themselves), either by increasing the monitorability of their outputs to the collective effort or by hav￾ing them work individually, then they would be motivated to work hard because of this potential evaluation. We are suggest￾ing that under certain conditions, the potential for a poor evalu￾ation of the group's performance will counteract the tendency to socially loaf. If a person expects that his or her co-worker will not perform well, then the person must go beyond his or her normal level of performance so that the group product and its evaluation will be respectable. Thus, the social compensation effect adds an additional piece to the puzzle of how individuals behave when working collectively. Possible Motives for Social Compensation We are not suggesting that the individual is concerned only with the direct evaluation of his or her own contribution, as is concluded in social loafing and social facilitation research. In￾stead, working on collective tasks without being individually identifiable compels individuals to be concerned for the evalua￾tion of the group. The group evaluation is their only means of obtaining a diffused individual evaluation, and this concern may result in social compensation when one's co-worker is not expected to adequately contribute. This view is consistent with Goethals and Darley's (1987) notion that people use their group's evaluation as a means of obtaining self-validation. Although we are suggesting that social compensation pri￾marily occurs because of people's concern for a favorable evalua￾tion of their performance, there are other possible motives that could lead to social compensation. One such motive involves altruistic concerns. Participants may want to protect their co￾workers from a poor evaluation, and thus may compensate when the co-worker is loafing. Future studies could manipulate the attractiveness of the co-worker or the cohesiveness of the co-workers to test these motives. The second motive suggests that when participants are work￾ing collectively with a loafing co-worker, they have something to gain and little to lose in terms of their own self-evaluation. If the collective product is poor, the participant can blame his or her co-worker; if the collective product is good, the participant can take credit for it. This might motivate the participant to work harder. On the other hand, when participants are working with a high effort co-worker, they have something to lose and little to gain. If the collective product is good, then the participant can￾not take credit for it; if the collective product is poor, then the participant must assume the blame. This might demotivate the participant. In expectancy-value terms, a favorable self-evalua￾tion is more contingent on individuals' performances when they are working with poorly performing co-workers, because they are more likely to attribute the group's performance to their own inputs. Attempts could be made in subsequent re￾search to separate concern for external evaluation (e.g., from the experimenter) from concern for self-evaluation. Although these explanations may be plausible causes for so￾cial compensation in some collective conditions, we think it is likely that concern for the experimenter's evaluation was more salient to our participants than was their concern for the well￾being of the experimenter or co-worker, or their concern for their own self-evaluation. 8 Of course, the broader body of small group literature suggests that groups can have positive effects on performance (e.g., Hackman & Morris, 1975; Laughlin & McGlynn, 1986; Moreland & Levine, 1990). In most of these studies, however, group performance was the primary focus, and even when individual effort was measured, it could not be separated from other factors such as incoordination

点击下载完整版文档(PDF)VIP每日下载上限内不扣除下载券和下载次数;
按次数下载不扣除下载券;
24小时内重复下载只扣除一次;
顺序:VIP每日次数-->可用次数-->下载券;
共12页,试读已结束,阅读完整版请下载
相关文档

关于我们|帮助中心|下载说明|相关软件|意见反馈|联系我们

Copyright © 2008-现在 cucdc.com 高等教育资讯网 版权所有