当前位置:高等教育资讯网  >  中国高校课件下载中心  >  大学文库  >  浏览文档

《社会心理学》课程教学资源(文献资料)conformity——Actors Conform, Observers React - The Effects of Behavioral Synchrony on Conformity

资源类别:文库,文档格式:PDF,文档页数:16,文件大小:197.7KB,团购合买
点击下载完整版文档(PDF)

置芳内a 0e2.351ss924 Actors Conform,Observers React: The Effects of Behavioral Synchrony on Conformity Uno XacaPandRoeta Engaging in synchr as behavior can induce a m eral dis others,which increa ity,c nally engas or might scem u it.Five experiments confirmed these opposing effects and circum are like r or merely can induce .Ih actors of group cohesi chess (Hai dt.2007 and these。 tntionnasitu acted (2010:Terry 2 1996).As our ngs indic howeve engaging behavio ected later At the same In the alled.the h restricts individuals'freedom of movement.can elicit psycholog- n synchrony with others,they focus their attention on this objec ive and the efe behavior (copying irected beh or may induce a behavioral r1,2014 012)that,once Jr..D CUHK D 2070488. is directed.Therefore.they are more likely to focus their attention on the actors"behavior itself independently of this goal.To this 60

Actors Conform, Observers React: The Effects of Behavioral Synchrony on Conformity Ping Dong University of Toronto Xianchi Dai and Robert S. Wyer Jr. The Chinese University of Hong Kong Engaging in synchronous behavior can induce a more general disposition to copy others, which increases the tendency to conform to others’ preferences in an unrelated choice situation. In contrast, observing others perform synchronous behavior can induce psychological reactance and decrease conformity to others’ preferences. Five experiments confirmed these different effects and circumscribed the conditions in which they occurred. Actors typically focus their attention on the goal to which their synchronous behavior is directed, inducing a copying-others mindset that generalizes to later situations. In contrast, observers focus on the actors’ behavior independently of the goal to which it pertains. Consequently, they become sensitive to the restrictions on freedom that synchronous behavior requires and experience reactance. However, changing the relative attention that actors and observers pay to these factors can reverse the effects of the actors’ synchronous behavior on conformity. Keywords: synchrony, conformity, actor– observer difference, reactance Synchronous behavior (i.e., behavior that matches others’ ac￾tions in time; Hove & Risen, 2009) pervades many aspects of daily life. Soldiers march in step; choir members sing in unison; dancers perform the same actions simultaneously. In some situations, we personally engage in this behavior. In other situations, we merely observe it. In either case, synchronous behavior might seem un￾likely to influence our judgments and decisions in situations that are quite unrelated to this behavior. In fact, however, this influence occurs. Moreover, the influence can be either positive or negative, depending on whether we actually perform the synchronous be￾havior or merely observe it. Why is this so? There are several possible answers to this question. In some cases, synchronous behavior can induce feelings of group cohesiveness (Haidt, 2007), and these feelings, in turn, can increase conformity to the individuals with whom one has interacted (Crane & Platow, 2010; Terry & Hogg, 1996). As our findings indicate, however, engaging in synchronous behavior might activate a more general disposition to copy others that affects judgments and decisions in a later situation independently of any feelings of social connectedness that might accompany it. At the same time, being required to copy others’ behavior, which restricts individuals’ freedom of movement, can elicit psycholog￾ical reactance (Brehm, 1966), motivating persons to reassert their individuality by not conforming. These two dispositions, which have conflicting implications for the adoption of others’ preference in an unrelated decision task, occur to different degrees in persons who engage in synchronous behavior and those who only observe it. Five experiments confirmed these opposing effects and circum￾scribed the conditions in which they occur. Our predictions were guided in part by evidence that actor– observer differences in perspective can affect the aspects of a situation to which individuals are likely to attend. Jones and Nisbett (1971) postulated that people are likely to focus their attention on the aspects of a situation that are salient to them from the perspective they consider it. Thus, actors’ attention in a situ￾ation is directed outward, on the goal to which their behavior is relevant and the instrumentality of this behavior in attaining this goal. In contrast, people who observe the actors are likely to focus on the actors’ behavior independently of the goal to which it is directed. In the conditions we investigated, these considerations suggest that when actors have an externally imposed objective of behaving in synchrony with others, they focus their attention on this objec￾tive and the effectiveness of their behavior (copying others’ ac￾tions) in attaining it. As we elaborate presently, concepts activated by performing this goal-directed behavior may induce a behavioral mindset (Wyer & Xu, 2010; Wyer, Xu, & Shen, 2012) that, once activated, increases conformity to others’ behavior in other, unre￾lated situations. Furthermore, people’s focus on the goal to which their behavior is directed is likely to decrease the feelings of being restricted that they might experience if they thought about their behavior per se. In contrast, individuals who only observe synchronous behavior may not be concerned with the goal to which the actors’ behavior is directed. Therefore, they are more likely to focus their attention on the actors’ behavior itself independently of this goal. To this This article was published Online First December 1, 2014. Ping Dong, Rotman School of Management, University of Toronto; Xianchi Dai and Robert S. Wyer Jr., Department of Marketing, CUHK Business School, The Chinese University of Hong Kong. This research was supported by Research Grants Council of Hong Kong Grants GRF 443012 (CUHK), 443710 (CUHK), 640011, 452813, 493113, and CUHK Business School Direct Grant Allocation 2070488. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Ping Dong, Rotman School of Management, University of Toronto, 105 Saint George Street, Toronto, Ontario M5S 3E6, Canada. E-mail: ping.dong12@ rotman.utoronto.ca This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology © 2014 American Psychological Association 2015, Vol. 108, No. 1, 60 –75 0022-3514/15/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000001 60

SYNCHRONY AND CONFORMITY 61 heir con cern about potential restrict s on their own freedom of aved in synchron For instance.Wiltermuth (2012)showed ate's reques noise blast to another group o ed that r would i ing othe e in this behavior have the opposite her expected tha d who have knowledge about the ctors hange the nated r reversed by introd his in nd r-related conce ed ir Theoretical Background tsthat are also onc Goals Versus Goal-Directed Behavior Goals and the of atta glanski et al (200 xist ir s possibility For i nemory,each co h a series o te tha concept ald which oftwo animls has more of a physical he goal.The con compose a ing one of sever As tal (2002) h might be part of more than one plan and thus might be relevant eatures of a stimulus ire a dis sition to might be part of a plan for getting social app va a plan for getting a cor wer to an e ing about pro sitions with which an stin sitio between the goal o d with th hat to atta mindset that decre s the infl ce o ands. vior co epts that compose the plar chavi However.they we likely to b mportant in ed the behay sections in the a in s The Effect of Behavioral Mindsets En ing in the Engaging in synchronous behavior can rise sens ehavior required genera have behaved in synchrony with one another show stronger coo The s that underlie the impact of a mindse ration and soc in subs nble tho ynchrony increases participantssense of attachment to the 200)How ver.the unique in several cts.Fo group

extent, they may be particularly sensitive to the constraints on freedom that copying others’ actions requires, and this can increase their concern about potential restrictions on their own freedom of action (Andreoli, Worchel, & Folger, 1974). These feelings may motivate them to affirm their individuality by conforming less to others’ judgments and decisions in a later situation than they would otherwise. On the basis of these considerations, we expected that engaging in synchronous behavior would increase the likelihood of con￾forming to others’ judgments and decisions in a later, unrelated situation, whereas observing other persons engage in this behavior would typically have the opposite effect. We further expected that the difference between actors’ and observers’ reactions to synchro￾nous behavior could be eliminated or reversed by introducing situational factors that change the relative attention that individuals pay to (a) the actors’ behavior per se and (b) the goal of this behavior. In the following sections, we review the theoretical and empirical bases for these predictions and present five experiments that confirm their validity. Theoretical Background Goals Versus Goal-Directed Behavior Goals and the means of attaining them are cognitively linked. Kruglanski et al. (2002) postulated that plan-goal schemas exist in memory, each composed of a goal concept along with a series of behavioral concepts that refer to the actions involved in attaining the goal. The concepts that compose a schema vary in abstractness but can be activated by situation-specific features that exemplify them. As Kruglanski et al. (2002) noted, a behavioral concept might be part of more than one plan and thus might be relevant to the attainment of more than one goal. For example, copying others might be part of a plan for getting social approval, a plan for getting a correct answer to an exam question, and, in the research we report, a plan to achieve synchronous behavior. It is important to distinguish between the goal of synchronous behavior and the actions that are required in order to attain it (attending to and copying others). The goal concept is activated by external demands, and the behavior concepts that compose the plan (copying others, etc.) are activated spontaneously in the course of pursuing this goal. Although this distinction is subtle, it becomes important in conceptualizing the different effects of synchronous behavior on judgments and decisions, as noted in the next two sections. The Effect of Behavioral Mindsets Engaging in synchronous behavior can often give rise to a sense of group cohesiveness (Ehrenreich, 2006; Haidt, 2007; Hove & Risen, 2009; Paladino, Mazzurega, Pavani, & Schubert, 2010; Wiltermuth, 2012; Wiltermuth & Heath, 2009). Wiltermuth and Heath (2009) demonstrated in several experiments that people who have behaved in synchrony with one another show stronger coop￾eration and social attachment in subsequent tasks, even at the cost of personal profit. Hove and Risen (2009) also found that behaving in synchrony increases participants’ sense of attachment to the group. In these studies, however, participants’ feelings of affiliation were directed toward the same individuals with whom they had behaved in synchrony. For instance, Wiltermuth (2012) showed that participants who acted in synchrony with a confederate were more likely than other participants to comply with the confeder￾ate’s request to administer a noise blast to another group of participants. Thus, feelings of emotional connectedness with the confederate mediated the relationship between synchrony and compliance with his or her request. It is unclear from this research whether engaging in synchronous behavior would influence ac￾tions in subsequent, unrelated situations in which different persons are involved who have no knowledge about the actors’ past be￾havior. Nevertheless, a conceptualization of behavioral mindsets pro￾posed by Wyer and Xu (2010) suggests that this influence can indeed occur. This conceptualization is based in part on (a) the effects of knowledge accessibility (Förster & Liberman, 2007; Higgins, 1996; Wyer, 2008) and in part on (b) Kruglanski et al.’s (2002) analysis of the mental representation of goals, as noted earlier. To reiterate, a behavior-related concept can be contained in several plans, each of which pertains to a different goal. Conse￾quently, if pursuing one goal activates concepts that are also part of a plan for attaining a different goal, these concepts, once accessible in memory, are likely to be applied in deciding how to pursue this latter goal as well (for a more formal statement of this process, see Wyer et al., 2012). Several studies exemplify this possibility. For instance, deciding which of two animals has more of a physical attribute can activate a “which-to-choose” mindset, increasing the likelihood of purchas￾ing one of several snacks on sale after the experiment without considering the option of choosing nothing at all (Xu & Wyer, 2008). Individuals who have been induced to focus on global features of a stimulus acquire a general disposition to construe information in terms of abstract concepts that generalizes to a wide variety of unrelated situations (Förster & Dannenberg, 2010). Thinking about propositions with which one disagrees can stimu￾late one to refute the propositions’ validity. Once concepts asso￾ciated with this behavior become accessible in memory, they can induce a “counterarguing” mindset that decreases the influence of a persuasive message that one encounters later (Xu & Wyer, 2012). In each case, individuals were likely to be aware of the behavior they performed. However, they were less likely to be aware that the behavior they performed in attaining the first goal influenced the behavior they decided to apply in pursuing the second. The situation we considered in the present research may also exemplify this phenomenon. The goal of behaving in synchrony may activate a plan-goal schema, concepts of which include the attention to and copying the behavior of others. Engaging in the behavior required to attain this goal may therefore activate general concepts associated with copying others and induce a “copying others” mindset, leading individuals to conform to others’ opinions and behavior in a later situation in pursuit of a quite unrelated goal to which the concepts are also applicable. The cognitive processes that underlie the impact of a mindset resemble those that underlie the effects of knowledge accessibility more generally (Förster & Liberman, 2007; Higgins, 1996; Wyer, 2008). However, the processes are unique in several respects. For one thing, the concepts that mediate the effects of a behavioral This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. SYNCHRONY AND CONFORMITY 61

62 DONG,DAL.AND WYER mindset do not exist in isolation.Rather.they are specifie plan the the would not beactivated under behavior or only observe it. unrelated to the shema in which a of the Actor-Observer Differences in Reactions to Behavior avior is contained. s the the thers mindset are likely to ave not oealppoPl tting g on whether indi soal elevanc Their focusin um may be hether they personally engage in the behavior or only hat individuals happen to be purs for a review,see Fiske&Taylor,1991)Jones and Nisbett (1971) ndivi postulatehatndrvidals attribute othe ior to factors tha this behavior Th tors'attention when the factors com into play.Reactance may be hey ar in atta typically drawn to Effects of Psvchological Reactance that e for hesp that of other s Res on fre n Molouk shes then and b engupta.2011) ed differed from these.In Ex ts to con vince a to on ome part ants engaged in a s es of exercise r synch ecrease rath ed a product evaluation question that a pro tion.they their conformi najority-ende P also induce For examplc. 4:Wang.Zhu.&Shiv,2012:Xu et al.2012).In administering I close to e task (Xu eir judgments That is if n ple feel that behaving or alter How they may tha from those that others der desirable le therefore expected that with the s primar ns go (1974)found that me seeing another person's freedom uct-choice less of whether the observers expected to interact with the person on the actors'behavior without regard to the goal to which i

mindset do not exist in isolation. Rather, they are contained in specific plan-goal schemas that are activated in the course of conscious goal-directed activity. In the present context, this means that a behavioral mindset would not be activated under conditions in which an individual’s behavior occurs for reasons that are unrelated to the plan-goal schema in which a concept of the behavior is contained. These considerations have general implications for the condi￾tions in which the effects of a copying-others mindset are likely to be evident. As we have noted, the concept of copying others might be part of a plan to attain goals other than behavior synchrony (social approval, getting a correct answer, etc.). The pursuit of these goals could also induce a copying-others mindset, thereby affecting judgments and decisions in other situations. On the other hand, if individuals incidentally copy others’ behavior in the course of pursuing a goal whose schema does not contain the concept of copying others, a copying-others mindset will not be activated and performing the behavior should have little effect. Moreover, even if copying others’ behavior is relevant to the goal that individuals happen to be pursuing, a copying-others mindset may not have an effect unless individuals think about the instru￾mentality of their behavior in attaining this goal. If individuals’ attention is drawn to their behavior per se, independently of its goal-relevance, other factors come into play. Reactance may be one of these factors. Effects of Psychological Reactance Consciously engaging in synchronous behavior requires indi￾viduals to sacrifice their freedom of action and subordinate their behavior to that of others. Restrictions on freedom can induce feelings of reactance (Brehm, 1966; Brehm & Brehm, 1981). According to reactance theory, people who feel that their freedom is threatened often attempt to reassert their individuality by engag￾ing in behavior that distinguishes themselves from others and by resisting social pressure to conform. For example, a person’s overly zealous attempts to convince a colleague to collaborate on a project can often decrease the colleague’s motivation to do so. Similarly, an unsolicited recommendation to see a movie might decrease rather than increase the likelihood of seeing the movie (Fitzsimons & Lehmann, 2004). Moreover, when people perceive that a promotion is intended to control their consumption, they reassert their individuality by choosing alternatives that are incon￾gruent with those the promotion advocates (Kivetz, 2005). Situational factors can also induce reactance. For example, restricting personal space by requiring persons to sit close to others can motivate them to reassert their freedom by choosing more unique alternatives in a later choice task (Xu, Shen, & Wyer, 2012). The requirement to engage in synchronous behavior could have analogous effects. That is, if people feel that behaving syn￾chronously threatens their freedom of action, they may reassert their individuality in a later situation by choosing actions that differ from those that others advocate or consider desirable. Moreover, reactance not only can be activated by restrictions on one’s own freedom but also can occur vicariously. Andreoli et al. (1974) found that merely seeing another person’s freedom being threatened decreased participants’ ratings of the desirability of stimuli that were ostensibly liked by others. This was true regard￾less of whether the observers expected to interact with the person who imposed the threat. In the present context, this suggests that people can feel the restrictions on freedom imposed by synchro￾nous behavior regardless of whether they personally engage in this behavior or only observe it. Actor–Observer Differences in Reactions to Behavior As the preceding discussion indicates, the cognitive and moti￾vational factors that come into play when individuals encounter synchronous behavior could have either positive or negative ef￾fects on their later judgments and decisions. In fact, both effects could occur, their relative magnitude depending on whether indi￾viduals focus their attention on the goal to which the behavior is directed or focus on the behavior itself, independently of its goal relevance. Their attentional focus, in turn, may be influenced in part by whether they personally engage in the behavior or only observe it. This possibility is suggested by research on actor– observer differences in attributions and judgments (Jones & Nisbett, 1971; for a review, see Fiske & Taylor, 1991). Jones and Nisbett (1971) postulate that individuals attribute others’ behavior to factors that are salient to them from the perspective from which they view or imagine this behavior. Thus, actors’ attention when they perform goal-related behavior is directed outward, on the goal they are pursuing and their behavior’s effectiveness in attaining it, whereas observers’ attention is typically drawn to characteristics of the actors’ behavior per se. These differences in perspective can in￾fluence the explanations that people give for the actors’ behavior (Regan & Totten, 1975; Storms, 1973). They can also have an impact on individuals’ perceptions of their own versus others’ conformity (Pronin, Berger, & Molouki, 2007), their self￾perceptions (Libby, Shaeffer, Eibach, & Slemmer, 2007), the in￾tensity of their emotional reactions (Hung & Mukhopadhyay, 2012), and their reliance on concrete versus abstract information (Yan & Sengupta, 2011). The effects we examined differed from these. In Experiment 1, some participants engaged in a series of exercises either synchro￾nously or asynchronously, and this behavior was observed by others. Then, as part of an ostensibly unrelated experiment, all participants completed a product evaluation questionnaire in which other consumers’ preferences for the products were indicated, and their conformity was inferred from the likelihood of choosing majority-endorsed options (for the use of this measure in prior research, see Berger & Heath, 2007; Huang, Zhang, Hui, & Wyer, 2014; Wang, Zhu, & Shiv, 2012; Xu et al., 2012). In administering these measures, we told participants to report their personal pref￾erences without giving them an indication of the criteria they should use. Therefore, their judgments could be based in part on their perceptions that others’ preference were indications of prod￾uct quality or, alternatively, social desirability. However, a copying-others mindset should have an impact on their conformity over and above these motivational factors. We therefore expected that participants with the goal of engag￾ing in synchronous exercise would focus primarily on this goal and the effectiveness of their behavior in attaining it. To this extent, the concepts activated by their behavior should induce a copying￾others mindset, leading them to copy others’ preferences in the product-choice task. In contrast, observers are more likely to focus on the actors’ behavior without regard to the goal to which it This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. 62 DONG, DAI, AND WYER

SYNCHRONY AND CONFORMITY ence reactance for rea suggested by Andreoli( themselves performing the beh uall expected that actorsbehavior was synchronous than when it was not izing the phe na we Additional Implications ltematively,whether other goals that lea It is conceivable that per it o r in attaining the goal to which e'h releva hus actors their atte should be om not nec al at er e to co equire cop ing one her's beh or ted To this both actors and observer eactions to the synchro s on a product preference task,to which cither the goal no efeets behavior activation more actions to the goal to which the or is directed,an our results are reported. o to which it pe Experiment 1 reely chose this activity hen they feel they were forced to ve that tcompletedaprodiuctcho which th f the behavior in oal relevan Second should theoreticall nption that observers tion of the reactions Wan of others med an altemative inter 1.2012)f el lonely sh should not be acti ated and a copying-others m hen they report the their later conformity.Experiment 3 provided evidence of this differ ereased conformity in this condition.As will be seen,however,this was not the cas ather than about the restrictions on their freedom required in order One hundred forty-six Hong Kong undergraduates (55 male 22.01 years.sp observers pay to the actors'behavior and the goal to which it is Nere randomly assigned to conditions of a 2 (behavior synehro nt 4 ous vs.asynchronous)(participant roe:actor vs.observer) ed that g observers to take tive reduced the reactance they experienced and led the actors'behav- nto two groups

pertains. To this extent, they should be sensitive to the restrictions on freedom imposed by the behavior and should vicariously ex￾perience reactance for reasons suggested by Andreoli et al. (1974). We therefore expected that observers would be less likely to conform to others’ opinions in the product-choice task when the actors’ behavior was synchronous than when it was not. Additional Implications The preceding hypotheses assume that actors and observers typically differ in the relative attention they pay (a) to the effec￾tiveness of the actors’ behavior in attaining the goal to which it is directed and (b) to the actors’ behavior per se, independently of its goal relevance. Thus, actors should be more likely to experience reactance if their attention is drawn to their behavior per se rather than to its instrumentality in attaining the goal at hand. Corre￾spondingly, observers should be less likely to experience reactance if their attention is drawn to the goal to which the actors’ behavior is directed. To this extent, several situational factors could influ￾ence both actors’ and observers’ reactions to the synchronous behavior through their mediating influence on (a) perceptions of the freedom to engage in the behavior, (b) the relevance of copying others’ actions to the goal to which their behavior is directed, and (c) the relative attention paid to the actors’ behavior per se versus the goal to which it pertains. Restrictions on freedom. Synchronous behavior is less likely to induce a negative reaction when individuals feel that they have freely chosen this activity than when they feel they were forced to engage in it (Brehm, 1966; see also Bem, 1972). Observers are also less likely to perceive that actors’ behavior constitutes a restriction on freedom if the actors volunteered to engage in the behavior. Thus, both actors and observers may express less reactance and, consequently, may be more likely to adopt others’ behavior in a later situation in the first case than in the second. Experiment 2 investigated this possibility. Goal relevance. Second, actors’ behavior should theoretically induce a copying-others mindset only if the actors consciously engage in the behavior for the purpose of matching their behavior to that of others. If actors incidentally match others’ behavior without intentionally doing so, a plan-goal schema that contains the concepts associated with copying others should not be acti￾vated and a copying-others mindset should not be induced. In this case, therefore, the actors’ synchronous behavior should not affect their later conformity. Experiment 3 provided evidence of this contingency. Attention differences. Although reactance and a copying￾others mindset can both contribute to actors’ conformity, the magnitude of these contributions may vary. To reiterate, actors are likely to think about their behavior in relation to the goal at hand rather than about the restrictions on their freedom required in order to attain the goal. In contrast, observers’ attention is more likely to be focused on the actors’ behavior per se. If this is so, however, the relative effects of this attention difference might be reduced or even reversed by changing the relative attention that actors and observers pay to the actors’ behavior and the goal to which it is directed. Experiments 4 and 5 confirmed this possibility. Experiment 4 showed that inducing observers to take the actors’ perspective reduced the reactance they experienced and led the actors’ behav￾ior to have an effect similar to its effects on actors themselves. Experiment 5 demonstrated that stimulating actors to imagine themselves performing the behavior in anticipation of actually doing so increased their sensitivity to the restrictions on freedom imposed by this behavior and thus decreased their later conformity. Other considerations arise in conceptualizing the phenomena we investigated. One question surrounds the extent to which the phenomena are specific to the goal of behaving synchronously or, alternatively, whether other goals that lead individuals to work together in pursuit of a common goal would have similar effects. It is conceivable that persons who interact with others in pursuit of a common goal will acquire a disposition to cooperate with others in a later situation. As we have noted, however, pursuit of a common goal does not necessarily involve copying others’ behav￾ior. (In fact, due to control conditions of the present research, actors had a common goal of doing the exercises; however, attain￾ment of the goal did not require copying one another’s behavior.) Thus, the pursuit of such a goal would not elicit a copying-others mindset that influences conformity to anonymous others’ judg￾ments on a product preference task, to which neither the goal nor the behavior required to attain it is relevant. We discuss the relative effects of goal activation and behavior activation more fully after our results are reported. Experiment 1 Experiment 1 confirmed the diametrically opposite effects of synchronous behavior on actors’ and observers’ judgments. Some participants were asked to perform exercises either synchronously or asynchronously. Other participants observed these exercises. Then, all participants completed a product choice task in which the market share of the choice alternatives was varied. We expected that engaging in synchronous behavior would increase the dispo￾sition to choose products that were ostensibly popular whereas observing this behavior would decrease this disposition. In addition to confirming this hypothesis, we (a) validated our assumption that observers’ reactions were mediated by thoughts about the restrictions on actors’ freedom of behavior and (b) disconfirmed an alternative interpretation of these reactions. Wang et al. (2012) found that persons who feel lonely show a preference for popular choice alternatives when they report these preferences publicly. (They show a preference for unique and distinctive alternatives when they report their judgments privately.) Judg￾ments in the present experiment were known only to the experi￾menter. Nevertheless, we expected that if observers of synchro￾nous behavior feel excluded from the main experiment and thus feel socially alienated, these feelings might account for their de￾creased conformity in this condition. As will be seen, however, this was not the case. Method One hundred forty-six Hong Kong undergraduates (55 male, Mage 22.01 years, SD 2.90) were recruited to participate voluntarily in exchange of HK$40 (approximately US$5). They were randomly assigned to conditions of a 2 (behavior: synchro￾nous vs. asynchronous) 2 (participant role: actor vs. observer) between-subjects design. Students participated in groups of 10 –12. Upon their arrival at the laboratory, participants were randomly divided into two groups This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. SYNCHRONY AND CONFORMITY 63

64 DONG,DAL,AND WYER of qu size and were told that we had a different task for each group was as were instructed to folle an expo to leam four ting that the s in s the Durbin-Wat oefficients ranged from 1 55 them to count aloud as they perfommed each movement.In asy M)d( is exercis nditio rthe the ne nature of the study design roo cepts models)The conc ear the the rswere given. HLM remained unchanged from conc ions base 'safphhcakBehevioalnchoayNesmanipulaied and were presented with nM=317.SD=1.10F1.142)=19.11D 001 Reporis about the asked to ch of three brands in ch of f vcs(4.00s.330.F1,142)=7.99p<01.F1. e undoubtedly individual differe in the or p who (In the case of sot 719 other indiv d a sofa from Wildor om Sky Fumiture an r actual bet he on whether s chose the n the indic es that the ma conformit nd participar data yielded s,P( 42) d the ming the exer ha hem feel that th chronous cond (M th in asy to which m and(b)the extent to wh ng the e 97)than in the latter (M 10.60.3 cported the hich they felt s that actor vely correlated with thei eg ch the id th 10)w all)to 5 (ven ch). ally ac d the ively correlated with their pe ceptions that the actors beha .14.D 0).The t t that these correlations synchronously.along the same scale used for other items Table I Results Actors'and Observers'Conformity asa Function of Behavioral Saatistical stratee The design of this and sub Synchrony:Experiment iments t me ough a within or obse Ro Syn Asynchronou g part o B Actor 193 measures in all five experiments.This coefficient,calculated for

of equal size and were told that we had a different task for each group. On this pretense, one group was assigned to be actors and the other to be observers. In synchronous exercise conditions, actors were instructed to follow an experimenter to learn four simple exercises involving head, arm, waist, and leg movements. They were told to do the exercises in synchrony with other actors (i.e., to perform the same actions in the same way at the same pace as others). To facilitate their attainment of this objective, we asked them to count aloud as they performed each movement. In asyn￾chronous exercise conditions, the procedure was the same except that actors were not instructed to do the exercises synchronously. While this was going on, the observers waited in another room and could not hear the instructions the actors were given. After the actors had practiced for 2 minutes, the observers reentered the experimental room and watched the actors perform for 10 minutes. All participants then moved to an ostensibly unrelated study (con￾ducted by a different researcher) in which they completed a prod￾uct preference questionnaire similar to that employed by Berger and Heath (2007). Participants were instructed that the researchers were interested in consumers’ preferences and were presented with information allegedly excerpted from Consumer Reports about the market share of different brands. Under this guise, participants were asked to choose one of three brands in each of five different product categories (e.g., car navigation system, sunglasses, and sofa). The alternatives in each category varied in terms of the proportion of persons who owned each. (In the case of sofa, for example, participants were told that, according to an online con￾sumer survey, 71% of other individuals owned a sofa from Wildon Home, 19% owned one from Skyline Furniture, and 10% owned one from Catnapper.) Participants’ choices were coded as 1, 2, or 3 depending on whether participants chose the option with the lowest, middle, or highest market share, respectively. Choices were summed over items to provide a score from 5 to 15, with higher numbers indicating a greater tendency to copy others’ preferences. After the brand choice task, actors indicated the extent to which performing the exercises had made them feel that their freedom of behavior was restricted, whereas observers reported both (a) the extent to which performing the exercises restricted the actors’ freedom and (b) the extent to which observing the exercise made them think about restrictions on their own freedom. In addition, observers reported the extent to which they felt socially excluded and the extent to which they felt they were not an important part of the experiment. All judgments were reported along scales from 1 (not at all) to 5 (very much). Finally, actors reported the extent to which they felt that they did the exercise synchronously, and observers indicated whether they felt the actors did the exercise synchronously, along the same scale used for other items. Results Statistical strategy. The design of this and subsequent exper￾iments meant that the current dependent measures, although as￾sessed individually, was nested within actors’ or observers’ groups. To examine the degree of variability in responses at the individual level that is attributed to being part of the group, we computed the intraclass correlation coefficient, ICC (1) (Bartko, 1976; James, 1982; Kozlowski & Klein, 2000), for the dependent measures in all five experiments. This coefficient, calculated for both the main dependent variables (conformity) and the great majority of other variables in our experiments, was typically small in size and nonsignificant (ranging from .13 to .11, ps  .10), suggesting that the effects of nesting were minimal and the data could be analyzed at the individual level. Furthermore, the Durbin–Watson coefficients ranged from 1.55 to 2.09 for all the analyses across the five experiments, suggesting that the independence assumption for the generalized linear model (GLM) was satisfied (see Durbin & Watson, 1950, 1971; Garson, 2012). Nevertheless, given the nested nature of the study design, we re-ran the regressions testing all hypotheses using hierarchical linear modeling (HLM; random intercepts models). The conclu￾sions based on HLM remained unchanged from conclusions based on the GLM approach. Manipulation check. Behavioral synchrony was manipulated successfully. Analyses of perceptions of synchrony as a function of behavior and participant role indicated that participants perceived a higher degree of synchrony in the synchronous exercise condi￾tion (M 3.91, SD 0.94) than in the asynchronous exercise condition (M 3.17, SD 1.10), F(1, 142) 19.11, p  .001, 2 .12, and that this difference did not depend on whether participants were actors (3.84 vs. 3.06), F(1, 142) 11.41, p .001, or observers (4.00 vs. 3.30, F(1, 142) 7.99, p  .01), F(1, 142) .06, p  .81. That said, there are undoubtedly individual differences in the degree to which actors were successful in engaging in synchronous behavior. Data pertaining to these differences are unavailable. However, our conceptualization pertains to individuals’ percep￾tions of their behavior and not their actual behavior. The fact that observers, like actors, perceived differences in the behavior in the two conditions indicates that the manipulation was successful. Conformity. Participants’ conformity to others’ product choices is shown in Table 1 as a function of behavioral synchrony and participant role. Analyses of these data yielded a significant interaction of these variables, F(1, 142) 10.87, p .001, 2 .07. That is, actors copied others’ preferences to a greater extent in synchronous conditions (M 10.88, SD 2.34) than in asyn￾chronous conditions (M 9.77, SD 1.90), F(1, 142) 4.88, p  .05, whereas observers conformed less in the former condi￾tions (M 9.47, SD 1.97) than in the latter (M 10.60, SD 1.77), F(1, 142) 5.11, p  .05. Supplementary analyses indi￾cated that actors’ conformity was positively correlated with their perceptions of the degree to which they did the exercises synchro￾nously (r .17, p  .10), whereas observers’ conformity was negatively correlated with their perceptions that the actors behaved synchronously (r .14, p  .10). The fact that these correlations Table 1 Actors’ and Observers’ Conformity as a Function of Behavioral Synchrony: Experiment 1 Role Behavior Synchronous Asynchronous Actor 10.88 (2.34)a 9.77 (1.90)b,c Observer 9.47 (1.97)c 10.60 (1.77)a,b Note. Cells with unlike subscripts differ at p  .05. Standard deviations are indicated in parentheses. This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. 64 DONG, DAI, AND WYER

SYNCHRONY AND CONFORMITY for the effeet to ur.We tested this propsition more for- onsonaciorsbehaviorlficedommcdiaicdihenegatiecfectol he actors Actors did n Experiment 2 d the extent in the s ence reac edinord was overridden by the g an freed 01)and 29、D .05).Mor choice would decre se any residual fee ings c 25,p05;sce Figure 1).The d that if ered to sh their nfimed by b nce than they othe wise would.We therefore expected that weer Feelings of social exclusion The behv rtic of social on Thes e feelings did not de d nchronous exercise conditi s either as actors or as (M 2.33 ns,F ever actors'free ers n that the not an whereas in other h lusion ppreciably cor would b p>.9Lo een given fre of the 68 In conelus Method synchronous behavior typically increases actors'tendency to con- tely USS5).They were random imen.participants took part in the study in groups of 25*2 igure Mediating effects of reactance on observers'conform words or d s the regr ected.all p lline for m participants chose to do the

did not achieve statistical significance suggests that the goal of behaving synchronously, but not incidental synchrony, was essen￾tial for the effect to occur. We tested this proposition more for￾mally in Experiment 3. Perceptions of freedom. Actors did not report feeling that their freedom was significantly more restricted while performing synchronous exercises (M 3.58, SD 1.12) than while per￾forming asynchronous exercises (M 3.37, SD 1.03, F  1), suggesting that they experienced little reactance. In contrast, ob￾servers reported that performing the exercise task restricted the actors’ freedom to a greater extent in the synchronous condition (M 3.71, SD 1.09) than in the asynchronous condition (M 3.10, SD 1.00), F(1, 66) 5.69, p  .05, 2 .08, and thought about restrictions on their own behavior to a greater extent in the former condition (M 3.42, SD .86) than in the latter (M 2.87, SD 1.17), F(1, 66) 5.10, p  .05, 2 .07. To assess the mediating impact of reactance on observers’ behavior, we averaged responses to the latter two measures. Be￾havioral synchrony was associated with both this index (.34, p  .01) and conformity ( .29, p  .05). Moreover, the reactance measure was associated with conformity (.32, p  .01). When both factors were entered into the predictor of confor￾mity, however, the effect of behavioral synchrony was reduced to marginal significance ( .20, p .10), whereas the effect of reactance remained reliable (.25, p  .05; see Figure 1). The indirect effect of synchrony on observers’ conformity tendency through their perceptions of actors’ behavioral freedom was fur￾ther confirmed by bootstrapping (Preacher & Hayes, 2008): Based on 1,000 samples, the 95% confidence interval ranged between 0.0205 and 0.8756, excluding zero. Feelings of social exclusion. The effects of behavioral syn￾chrony on observers’ judgments cannot be attributed to their feelings of social exclusion. These feelings did not depend on whether the behavior they observed was synchronous (M 2.53, SD 1.11) or asynchronous (M 2.33, SD 1.09, F  1). Observers’ perception that they were not an important part of the experiment was also similar in synchronous (M 2.71, SD 1.11) and asynchronous conditions (M 2.60, SD 1.25, F  1). Finally, neither measure of social exclusion was appreciably cor￾related with observers’ conformity (for social exclusion feeling: r .01 p  .91; for perception that they were not an important part of the experiment: r .05, p  .68). Thus, this alternative interpretation of our findings does not seem tenable. In conclusion, this experiment confirmed our expectation that synchronous behavior typically increases actors’ tendency to con￾form while observing this behavior normally decreases observers’ conformity. Furthermore, observers’ thoughts about the restric￾tions on actors’ behavioral freedom mediated the negative effect of the actors’ synchronous behavior on their conformity, whereas observers’ feelings of social exclusion had no impact on their disposition to conform. Experiment 2 We assumed that both actors who engaged in synchronous behavior and observers of this behavior would experience reac￾tance to the restrictions on their freedom that was required in order to perform the behavior but that, in the case of actors, the effect of this reactance was overridden by the effect of a copying-others mindset. In fact, actors in Experiment 1 did not report feeling any more restrictions on their behavior when it was synchronous than when it was asynchronous, suggesting that they experienced little reactance despite being acquired to engage in synchronous behav￾ior. We nevertheless expected that explicitly giving them freedom of choice would decrease any residual feelings of reactance that they might otherwise experience and, therefore, would increase the relative impact of a copying-others mindset on their later behav￾ioral decisions. Correspondingly, we assumed that if observers were made aware that the actors volunteered to perform synchronous behav￾ior, they would be less likely to perceive the behavior to be a restriction on their freedom and would experience less vicarious reactance than they otherwise would. We therefore expected that giving actors freedom to choose whether to engage in synchronous behavior would increase both actors’ and observers’ disposition to copy others’ choices in a later situation. Experiment 2 examined this possibility. All participants were exposed to synchronous exercise conditions either as actors or as observers. In some conditions, however, actors’ freedom of choice was increased by asking them to choose between two alternative studies, whereas in other conditions, actors were not given any choice. We expected that although the general difference between actors’ and observers’ preferences might continue to exist, both actors and observers would be more inclined to select popular options in the product choice task when actors had been given free choice over their behavior than when they had not. Method One hundred seventeen Hong Kong undergraduates (48 male, Mage 20.67 years, SD 1.35) participated in this study for payment of HK$40 (approximately US$5). They were randomly assigned to conditions of a 2 (freedom of choice: free vs. forced) 2 (participant role: actor vs. observer) between-subjects design. As in Experiment 1, participants took part in the study in groups of 10 –12 persons and were randomly divided into actors and observ￾ers. In forced choice conditions, actors were required to do the exercise synchronously, and no alternative choices were provided. In free choice conditions, actors were asked to choose between two studies: the synchrony task and an essay writing task in which they would be asked to write an essay of about 500 words on “the individual and the cosmos in Renaissance philosophy.” As we expected, all participants chose to do the exercise task. Corre￾spondingly, observers either were told that the actors were required Behavioral Synchrony Reactance Conformity Tendency (Observers) .29* -.32** -.25* (.20ns) Figure 1. Mediating effects of reactance on observers’ conformity: Exper￾iment 1. The value in parentheses indicates the regression coefficient of behavioral synchrony on conformity tendency (observers) controlling for re￾actance. ns nonsignificant. p  .05. p  .01. This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. SYNCHRONY AND CONFORMITY 65

66 DONG,DAI,AND WYER been given a two aforementioned options and After the synchro in the case ofo how much freedom they had hen they do the exerc the Results egend o her ootep observers(s.224. rticipant role Analyses of these data revealed main situational reactance(B=-55.p<01)significantly predicted th pa re to oth 9590 o nonsignificance (B08.p37).whereas the effecto esting that the effect of choice freedom on mity wa reater dispo preferences under free choice confirm this con- red and averaged with their estimate of pere eived threa ingle index Insummary.the results of this experiment indicate that the 72) ns (M 322.SD d bo cho by affecting their psychological reactance. signi Experiment 3 ed the Our interpretation of the effect of actors'synchronous behavior and observers,both freedom of choice (B=29.p<01)and Table 2 heit behavior to others'behavior ind dently of its relevance to Effect of Natre of Choicen Actors'and Observers Subsequent Conformity Tendency:Experiment 2 Choice fredom of ynchrom or reas ving synch Role ac nd sho ce synchromy 89 19978欲 e als reich,2006;see also Haidt,207 Haidt,Seder.Kesebir,2008)

to do the synchronous behavior task or were told that the actors had been given a choice of the two aforementioned options and had chosen to perform the exercises. After the synchronous exercise, all participants completed the same product preference task employed in Experiment 1. In addi￾tion, they indicated how much threat the synchronous activities had posed to their behavioral freedom and how much freedom they experienced when they were doing the synchronous exercises (or, in the case of observers, how much freedom they had experienced when they watched the actors do the exercises). Finally, they indicated the extent to which the actors were forced to perform the synchrony task. All responses were made along a scale from 1 (not at all) to 5 (very much). Results Manipulation check. Analyses of perceptions of synchrony as a function of choice freedom and participant role indicated that the actors indeed had greater freedom of choice in free choice conditions than in forced choice conditions, F(1, 113) 122.20, p  .001, 2 .52, and this difference did not significantly depend on whether participants were actors (4.07 vs. 2.55), F(1, 113) 48.96, p  .001, or observers (4.10 vs. 2.24, F(1, 113) 74.71, p  .001), F(1, 113) 1.25, p  .26. Conformity. Participants’ conformity to others’ preferences is summarized in Table 2 as a function of freedom of choice and participant role. Analyses of these data revealed main effects of both participant role, F(1, 113) 9.24, p .003, 2 .08, and choice freedom, F(1, 113) 10.77, p .001, 2 .09. Actors generally conformed more to others’ preferences (M 9.59, SD 1.48) than observers did (M 8.71, SD 1.71), confirming the results of Experiment 1. However, both actors and observers had a greater disposition to copy others’ preferences under free choice conditions (M 9.62, SD 1.44) than under forced choice conditions (M 8.68, SD 1.73). Furthermore, the interaction of choice freedom and participant role was not significant (F  1). Reactance. Participants’ estimate of perceived freedom was reverse scored and averaged with their estimate of perceived threat to form a single index of reactance. As we expected, participants perceived greater reactance in forced choice conditions (M 3.88, SD 0.72) than under free choice conditions (M 3.22, SD 0.78), F(1, 113) 22.87, p  .001, 2 .17. Neither the main effect of participant role nor its interaction with choice freedom was significant (ps  .22). Regression analyses confirmed the mediating effect of reactance on the impact of choice freedom on conformity. Pooled over actors and observers, both freedom of choice ( .29, p  .01) and situational reactance (.55, p  .001) significantly predicted conformity. When both factors were entered as the predictor of conformity, however, the effect of freedom of choice was reduced to nonsignificance ( .08, p .37), whereas the effect of situational reactance remained reliable (.52. p  .001), suggesting that the effect of choice freedom on conformity was mediated by reactance. Bootstrapping further confirmed this con￾clusion: Based on 1,000 samples, the 95% confidence interval ranged from 0.3803 to 1.1604, excluding 0. Separate analyses of actors and observers, summarized in Figure 2, confirm this con￾clusion. In summary, the results of this experiment indicate that the freedom of choice over performing synchronous behavior influ￾enced both actors’ and observers’ adoption of others’ preferences by affecting their psychological reactance. Experiment 3 Our interpretation of the effect of actors’ synchronous behavior on their later behavior assumes that the actors consciously perceive their actions as a means of attaining the goal of behaving synchro￾nously. However, one might speculate that actors’ conformity in synchronous behavior conditions is induced by the similarity of their behavior to others’ behavior independently of its relevance to the goal to which the behavior was directed. If this were so, however, inducing actors to match others’ behavior incidentally, for reasons that are unrelated to the goal of behaving synchro￾nously, should not activate a copying-others mindset and should not influence their conformity in later situations. Experiment 3 confirmed this hypothesis. We also evaluated another possibility. Engaging in synchronous behavior can give rise to a sense of group cohesiveness (Ehren￾reich, 2006; see also Haidt, 2007; Haidt, Seder, & Kesebir, 2008) Table 2 Effect of Nature of Choice on Actors’ and Observers’ Subsequent Conformity Tendency: Experiment 2 Role Choice freedom of synchrony Forced synchrony Free-choice synchrony Actor 9.10 (1.57)a 10.07 (1.22)b Observer 8.27 (1.80)a 9.17 (1.51)b,c Note. Cells with unlike subscripts differ at p  .05. Standard deviations are indicated in parentheses. (A) Actor Group (B) Observer Group Choice Freedom Situational Reactance Conformity Tendency -.45** -.57** .31** (.11ns) Choice Freedom Situational Reactance Conformity Tendency -.67** -.69** .27* (.04ns) Figure 2. Mediating effects of situational reactance on the effect of choice freedom on actors’ and observers’ conformity: Experiment 2. The values in parentheses indicate the regression coefficient of choice freedom on confor￾mity tendency controlling for situational reactance. ns nonsignificant. p  .05. p  .01. This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. 66 DONG, DAI, AND WYER

SYNCHRONY AND CONFORMITY andconcaqucntcanncrceagcopentionandcelnesofocdl Results muth 2012 Wiltermuth&Heath 2009)In these earlier studie d the sam 5.38.SD=0.98)than in asynchrony conditions (M=3.69.SD= n each cas ,Fl,82)> .69,P13.Thus,particip ants in both Method 1) others perceived their objective 161)participated in groups They were randomly cach behavior.Th .92,p ed synchronous behavior und in time to this h not 9.39 participants performe tcahdgano perform the iors at own pa (p pleted the product pple attention to what other oing did t differ significantly in in ntion the aid to others'behavior ("l paid attention to were ing)(b)their tly as others were doing to a significa ater extent in th M feel tha was be 2.55,SD 124),F2,82)= 31.20 felt ng th ny)ind that both inte I synchrony cipants estim d the extent to wh h the felt that the did the n both dummy variables and the proposed mediator intentional svnchrony was reduced to nonsignificance (=14 to7(happ). =.36).whereas the effect of the goal to behave like others Asynchrony 9,79124 4.48(125 4.24(1.46. 359(127八 3.44 424 dicated in parenthese

and consequently can increase cooperation and feelings of social attachment (Hove & Risen, 2009; Paladino et al., 2010; Wilter￾muth, 2012; Wiltermuth & Heath, 2009). In these earlier studies, participants’ feelings of affiliation were directed toward the same individuals with whom they had previously interacted. It seemed intuitively unlikely that these feelings would mediate the effects of performing synchronous behavior on conformity to anonymous individuals who had not been engaged in this behavior. Neverthe￾less, this possibility was worth evaluating empirically. Method Eighty-five Canadian undergraduate students (29 male, Mage 19.89 years, SD 1.61) participated in groups of 6 to 8 persons each for course credit. They were randomly assigned to one of three conditions. In intentional synchrony conditions, participants engaged in synchronous behavior under conditions identical to those employed in Experiment 1. In incidental synchrony condi￾tions, participants learned the same four body movements with the experimenter but were then told to perform the exercises in time to a metronome. Thus, although their behavior was synchronized, performing this behavior was not their objective. In asynchrony conditions, participants performed the same behaviors but were told to perform the behaviors at their own pace. In all conditions, participants performed the exercises for around 10 minutes. After finishing the exercises, participants completed the product preference task administered in previous experiments. Then, they reported their agreement with several items pertaining to (a) the attention they paid to others’ behavior (“I paid attention to what others were doing”), (b) their goal while doing the exercise (“My goal was to do exercises exactly as others were doing them”), (c) the perceived restriction on their behavioral freedom (“Performing the exercise task made me feel that my behavior was being restricted”), and (d) their closeness and similarity to other partic￾ipants (“I felt personally close to other participants who were performing the exercises,” “I felt similar to other participants who were performing the exercises”; r .31, p  .01), all along a scale from 1 (disagree very much) to 7 (agree very much). Finally, participants estimated the extent to which they felt that they did the exercise synchronously along a scale from 1 (not at all) to 7 (very much) and reported their general mood along a scale from 1 (sad) to 7 (happy). Results Manipulation check. Our manipulation of behavioral syn￾chrony was successful. Participants reported their behavior to be more similar to others in both intentional synchrony conditions (M 5.81, SD 1.04) and incidental synchrony conditions (M 5.38, SD 0.98) than in asynchrony conditions (M 3.69, SD 1.20), in each case, F(1, 82)  35.69, p  .001, whereas judg￾ments in the two synchrony conditions did not differ from one another, F(1, 82) 2.29, p  .13. Thus, participants in both incidental synchrony and intentional synchrony conditions were aware that they had performed the exercises in synchrony with others. As noted presently, however, they perceived their objective of performing the behavior to differ in the two conditions. Conformity. Participants’ adoption of others’ preferences in each behavior condition is shown in the first row of Table 3. The effect of behavior conditions was significant, F(2, 82) 4.92, p  .05, 2 .11, and indicates that participants copied others’ pref￾erences significantly more in intentional synchrony conditions (M 10.96, SD 1.65) than in either incidental synchrony conditions (M 10.14, SD 1.38), F(1, 82) 4.67, p  .05, or asynchrony conditions (M 9.79, SD 1.24), F(1, 82) 9.39, p  .01, whereas conformity in the latter two conditions did not differ (F  1). Participants’ mood did not affect their conformity (p  .71). Supplementary data shown in the next two rows of Table 3 confirmed our assumptions concerning the factors that underlie these differences. Although participants’ attention to what others were doing did not differ significantly in intentional and incidental synchrony conditions, participants reported trying to do exercises exactly as others were doing to a significantly greater extent in the intentional synchrony condition (M 5.07, SD 1.24) than in either incidental synchrony conditions (M 4.10, SD 1.15) or asynchrony conditions (M 2.55, SD 1.24), F(2, 82) 31.20, p  .001, 2 .43. Mediation analyses involving all three behav￾ior conditions (we created dummy variables for intentional and incidental synchrony) indicated that both intentional synchrony ( .37, p  .01) and the “goal to behave exactly as others” ( .37, p  .001) were significantly related to conformity. When both dummy variables and the proposed mediator were entered into the predictor of conformity, however, the effect of intentional synchrony was reduced to nonsignificance ( .14, p .36), whereas the effect of the goal to behave like others Table 3 Actors’ Conformity and Process Measures as a Function of Behavioral Synchrony: Experiment 3 Measure Intentional synchrony Incidental synchrony Asynchrony F Conformity 10.96 (1.65)a 10.14 (1.38)b 9.79 (1.24)b 4.92 I paid attention to what others were doing. 5.26 (1.06)a 4.72 (1.13)a,b 4.48 (1.18)b 3.45 My goal was to do exercises exactly as others were doing them. 5.07 (1.24)a 4.10 (1.15)b 2.55 (1.24)c 31.20 Performing the exercise task made me feel that my behavior was being restricted. 4.48 (1.25)a 4.24 (1.46)a,b 3.59 (1.27)b 3.44 I felt personally close to other participants who were performing the exercises. 5.11 (1.09)a 4.34 (1.11)b 4.14 (1.36)b 5.12 I felt similar to other participants who were performing the exercises. 5.19 (1.04)a 4.55 (1.30)b 3.93 (1.03)c 8.58 Note. Cells with unlike subscripts in each row differ at p  .05. Standard deviations are indicated in parentheses. p  .05. p  .01. This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. SYNCHRONY AND CONFORMITY 67

DONG,DAL.AND WYER )Boots ence cem viable d in both intentional synchrony Experiment 4 5)and inc dental s Experiment3 showed that the synchronous behavioro 424 ns the no s a co d nsve to objective (Exp ment )If this is so.focusing observersatte Feelings of similarity and social closeness. Participants and reduce the reactance they experience old tha ast two rows of Table 3.Participants felt significantly closer their dition reward ould b conditions was medi .000 bo ctor.they should thecors behavior ind 95%CI based on 1 000 samples from -01578t0379g nce reactance as they did in exr ncluding 0)significantly mediated the effects of intentiona cces The results of this ent supoo r a tion that the ality of the a rs'behay thers he a of this come.of s judgment To do this.w Finally although behaving d actor feelings of closene t of their behavior on conformity to others Cons we expected that conformit te that the effects of actors synchronous behavior n their in previous Method med to conditions of a 2 (partic chavior/outcom rclatcdncs outcomes vs.asynchronous behavior) ned t iticno which each group's performance would be evaluated by two

remained reliable ( .30, p  .05). Thus, participants’ attempts to behave like others significantly mediated the effects of behavioral synchrony on their later conformity (see Figure 3). Bootstrapping confirmed this conclusion: Based on 1,000 samples, the 95% confidence interval ranged between 0.1020 and 0.9477, excluding 0. Reactance. Participants’ reactance is summarized in the fourth row of Table 3. Participants reported feeling that their behavior was more restricted in both intentional synchrony condi￾tions (M 4.48, SD 1.25) and incidental synchrony conditions (M 4.24, SD 1.46) than in asynchrony conditions (M 3.59, SD 1.27), F(2, 82) 3.44, p  .05, 2 .08. This difference confirms the nonsignificant difference observed in Experiment 1 and confirms our assumption that although actors who behaved synchronously were sensitive to restrictions on their freedom, the effects of a copying-others mindset overrode whatever effects their reactance had on their conformity. Feelings of similarity and social closeness. Participants’ feelings of closeness and similarity to others are shown in the last two rows of Table 3. Participants felt significantly closer and more similar to others in intentional synchrony conditions than in other conditions. This raises the possibility that partic￾ipants’ greater conformity in the former conditions was medi￾ated by their feelings of closeness to others. In fact, however, mediation analyses indicated that neither feelings of social closeness (95% CI based on 1,000 bootstrapping samples, from 0.2804 to 0.2940, including 0) nor feelings of similarity (95% CI based on 1,000 samples, from 0.1578 to 0.3799, including 0) significantly mediated the effects of intentional synchrony on conformity. The results of this experiment support our assumption that the conscious objective of behaving synchronously with others is necessary to induce a copying-others mindset. Merely behaving similarly to others without having the conscious objective of doing so is not sufficient. The failure for the actors’ behavior to influence their conformity in incidental synchrony conditions suggests that the effects did not result from a disposition to unconsciously mimic others’ behavior (Chartrand & Bargh, 1999) that general￾izes over situations. Finally, although behaving synchronously increased actors’ feelings of closeness and similarity to the partic￾ular persons with whom they interacted, this factor did not mediate the impact of their synchronous behavior on conformity to others’ judgments in an unrelated situation. Incidentally, one might speculate that the effects of actors’ synchronous behavior on their conformity in previous experiments resulted in part from the presence of observers, who might have distracted them from thinking about the restrictions on freedom imposed by their behavior. In the present study, however, actors conformed in intentional synchrony conditions even in the absence of observers. Therefore, this alternative interpretation does not seem viable. Experiment 4 Experiment 3 showed that the effect of synchronous behavior on actors’ conformity results from their disposition to focus attention on the effectiveness of their behavior in copying others’ actions. As a consequence, actors were relatively insensitive to the con￾straints on their freedom that were required in order to attain this objective (Experiment 1). If this is so, focusing observers’ atten￾tion on the effectiveness of the actors’ behavior in attaining this goal might also desensitize them to the constraints on this behavior and reduce the reactance they experience. Experiment 4 investigated this possibility. Actors were told that their success in behaving synchronously would be compared to that of other groups of participants and that a reward would be given to the group that was most successful. Moreover, in some conditions, observers were told that they would share the reward with the actors, thus tying their own outcomes to the actors’ success. When observers’ own outcomes are not linked to those of actors, they should typically focus on the actors’ behavior inde￾pendently of the goal to which the behavior was directed and should vicariously experience reactance as they did in Experiment 1. When observers’ outcomes are contingent on the actors’ success in behaving synchronously, however, we expected observers to focus their attention on the instrumentality of the actors’ behavior to the attainment of this outcome, offsetting the reactance that otherwise predominated and increasing the likelihood of adopting others’ judgments in a later situation to a level similar to that observed when actors’ behavior was asynchronous. In addition, we increased the generalizability of our earlier findings to actual behavior as well as judgments. To do this, we used a different index of conformity: namely, the donation of money to either well-known or lesser known charities (see Levav & Zhu, 2009). We assumed that participants would perceive the well-known charities to be more popular than the lesser known ones. Consequently, we expected that individuals’ conformity would be reflected in a greater difference between their donations to the former charities and their donations to the latter ones. Method One hundred forty Hong Kong undergraduates (51 male, Mage 20.06 years, SD 1.46) participated for course credit. Participants were randomly assigned to conditions of a 2 (partic￾ipant role: actor vs. observer) 3 (behavior/outcome relatedness: synchronous behavior/dependent outcomes vs. synchronous be￾havior/independent outcomes vs. asynchronous behavior) between-subjects design. As in Experiment 1, individuals participated in groups of 10 –12, with half of them assigned to be actors and the others assigned to be observers. In synchronous behavior conditions, actors were told that (a) we were holding a “synchronous exercise” competition in which each group’s performance would be evaluated by two Intentional Synchrony Goal to Behave Like Others Conformity Tendency (Actors) .75** .37** .30* (.14ns) Figure 3. Mediating effect goal on actors’ conformity: Experiment 3. The value in parentheses indicates the regression coefficient of intentional syn￾chrony on conformity tendency (actors) controlling for goal to behave like others. ns nonsignificant. p  .05. p  .001. This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. 68 DONG, DAI, AND WYER

SYNCHRONY AND CONFORMITY ts behavior condition actors performed their Finally,participants rated the synchrony of the exercises the cted to d HK$30 cash y,and the rved along a s age performance of individuals in their group high which they perceived both actors and observers s to be“in-grour of obs rs Results and act first in of th ewards that a receive i ave b D=0.5 synchronous behay M= roup they were ing of the a F(1.136)=)and they ere told tha rewards 95.F1.136 127.27.D 001):the inte ctio was no winning actor group. to that fomity by Levav 0.76,F1,70)=80.04,p<001,m 53.Similarly,observers hat th ons.They were told that they ha 510 in more.the Donation to othe nation be and the Breas Cane charities (to which other were likel onate)an wn ch were less e ged from SHK2 to SHK19 n type The articip sand partici pant role ence scores yielded main effects o n pa and oh tion to Donate to Well-Known Versus Lesser Known Charities as a Function of Behavioral Synchrony and Outcome Independence:Experiment4 known charities 638批 68838 282 wn charities 39 号888品 487跳 品8邵 478提 -88款

independent judges and (b) each member of the winning group would receive a cash reward of HK$30 (approximately US$4). In asynchronous behavior conditions, actors performed their exer￾cises without being instructed to do them synchronously, and they were told that each of them would get a HK$30 cash prize if the average performance of individuals in their group was the highest. The criteria for judging synchronous behavior included the extent to which the behaviors are synchronous, whether the movements were correct, and the extent to which participants applied efforts to the exercises (the latter two criteria were used to judge the indi￾vidual performance of those in the asynchronous behavior condi￾tions). As a manipulation of observers’ perception of the tie between their outcomes and actors’ outcomes, observers were first in￾formed of the rewards that actors would receive in synchronous (asynchronous) behavior conditions. Then, in dependent-outcome conditions, they were told that if the group they were observing won, they would attain a reward equivalent to that of the actors. In both independent-outcome and asynchronous behavior conditions, however, they were told that rewards would be allocated to actors alone but that, to be fair, each group of observers would be entered into a lottery and that the lottery-winning group would receive a reward equivalent to that of the winning actor group. After this exercise stage, all participants completed a donation task similar to that used to assess conformity by Levav and Zhu (2009). Participants were given HK$10 (approximately US$1.25) that they could use for donating money to a list of six nonprofit organizations. They were told that they could decide to give any amount they wished to each organization. If they donated less than $10 in total, they could keep the rest for themselves, and if they wanted to donate more, they could do so using their own money. Three of the organizations (the Red Cross, the Society for Protec￾tion of Children, and the Breast Cancer Foundation) were well known in Hong Kong, whereas the other three (AIDS Organiza￾tion, the Down Syndrome Association, and the Arthritis and Rheu￾matism Foundation) were less known. This difference was con￾firmed on the basis of pretesting. Thirty participants from the same subject population rated the organizations’ familiarity along a scale from 1 (not at all) to 7 (very). These participants were significantly more familiar with the three well-known charities (M 3.52, SD 1.50) than with the three lesser known ones (M 2.21, SD 0.97), F(1, 29) 38.07, p  .001, 2 .57. Finally, participants rated the synchrony of the exercises they performed or observed along a scale from 1 (not at all synchro￾nous) to 5 (very synchronous). Then, they reported the extent to which they perceived both actors and observers to be “in-group” members along a scale from 1 (not at all) to 5 (very much). Participants were then debriefed and dismissed. The money col￾lected in this study was actually sent to the specific charities that the participants had designated. Results Manipulation checks. Participants judged the exercise to have been performed more synchronously in synchronous behav￾ior conditions (M 3.80, SD 0.53) than in asynchronous behavior conditions (M 2.00, SD 0.45), F(1, 136) 355.63, p  .001, 2 .72, and this was equally true of both actors (3.80 vs. 2.05, F(1, 136) 142.31, p  .001) and observers (3.79 vs. 1.95, F(1, 136) 127.27, p  .001); the interaction was not significant, F(1, 136) .20, p  .65. Actors were more inclined to perceive both themselves and observers to be in-group members when the observers’ outcome depended on their performance (M 3.76, SD 0.72) than when it did not (M 2.11, SD 0.76), F(1, 70) 80.04, p  .001, 2 .53. Similarly, observers were more inclined to perceive both themselves and actors to be in-group members in the former condition (M 3.43, SD 0.59) than in the latter (M 1.78, SD 0.56), F(1, 66) 128.71, p  .001, 2 .66. These differences did not depend on whether the actors’ behavior was synchronous or asynchronous (ps  .20). Donations. Participants’ conformity to others’ donation be￾havior was inferred from the difference between their donations to well-known charities (to which others were likely to donate) and their donations to lesser known charities (to which others were less likely to donate). Seven participants did not contribute anything. Other participants’ donations ranged from $HK2 to $HK19. These difference scores, along with the mean donations to each type of charity separately, are summarized in Table 4 as a function of behavior/outcome relatedness and participant role. Analyses of the difference scores yielded main effects of both participant role, F(1, Table 4 Actors’ and Observers’ Disposition to Donate to Well-Known Versus Lesser Known Charities as a Function of Behavioral Synchrony and Outcome Independence: Experiment 4 Variable Behavior/outcome relatedness Synchronous behavior/dependent outcome Synchronous behavior/independent outcome Asynchronous behavior Well-known charities Actor 6.12 (2.43)a 6.85 (2.38)a 4.29 (2.57)b Observer 5.63 (2.56)a,b 3.16 (2.29)c 5.65 (2.06)b Lesser known charities Actor 3.00 (2.52)a 2.58 (2.02)a 4.38 (2.75)b Observer 3.72 (2.38)a,b 5.68 (2.61)c 4.10 (1.74)b Difference Actor 3.12 (3.61)a 4.27 (3.45)a 0.10 (4.17)b Observer 1.91 (4.41)a,b 2.52 (4.24)c 1.55 (2.68)b Note. Cells with unlike subscripts differ at p  .05. Standard deviations are indicated in parentheses. This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. SYNCHRONY AND CONFORMITY 69

点击下载完整版文档(PDF)VIP每日下载上限内不扣除下载券和下载次数;
按次数下载不扣除下载券;
24小时内重复下载只扣除一次;
顺序:VIP每日次数-->可用次数-->下载券;
共16页,试读已结束,阅读完整版请下载
相关文档

关于我们|帮助中心|下载说明|相关软件|意见反馈|联系我们

Copyright © 2008-现在 cucdc.com 高等教育资讯网 版权所有