o品8P Continuity and Change in Self-Esteem During Emerging Adulthood Richad W.Robins. wEak品aty uKetcMidmatoe mple of em d.at th r 4th the d hey thoug 7).0n during the Ist 16)but sign trom the vith re yhigh self- sel vith actual incr Keywords:self-estecm.self-perceived change.emerging adulthood.college transition Global se-emrefersto n subjective eval societal goa and across contexts(T skI,D n.Ro bins.2003 Ahough conducted to date has emphasized the opins esponsive toevents】 tance abuse, and antisocial beha (e.g.Crocker& first time.begin college and full-time jobs.or marry and have Schneider,Larsen McClarty,2007:Trzesniewski et al.,2006). 6) 1:Erikson emergin adulthood ma be a time during which global sef e M.Ch and Ric rd W.Ro De likely toc of t.U of Ca tityand subsequent of conceptions and aua of the se of Ca cy and ch m during emerging adultho od.As in m on per we ve the ra M h examined predictors of the self-esteem trajectory.attempting to this article s hould be One indicators of success in the college context-academic achieve-
Continuity and Change in Self-Esteem During Emerging Adulthood Joanne M. Chung, Richard W. Robins, and Kali H. Trzesniewski University of California, Davis Erik E. Noftle Willamette University Brent W. Roberts University of Illinois, Urbana–Champaign Keith F. Widaman University of California, Davis The present study examined the development of self-esteem in a sample of emerging adults (N 295) followed longitudinally over 4 years of college. Six waves of self-esteem data were available. Participants also rated, at the end of their 4th year, the degree to which they thought their self-esteem had changed during college. Rank-order stability was high across all waves of data (Mdn disattenuated correlation .87). On average, self-esteem levels dropped substantially during the 1st semester (d .68), rebounded by the end of the 1st year (d .73), and then gradually increased over the next 3 years, producing a small (d .16) but significant mean-level increase in self-esteem from the beginning to the end of college. Individuals who received good grades in college tended to show larger increases in self-esteem. In contrast, individuals who entered college with unrealistically high expectations about their academic achievement tended to show smaller increases in self-esteem, despite beginning college with relatively high self-esteem. With regard to perceived change, 67% reported that their self-esteem increased during college, whereas 12% reported that it declined; these perceptions tended to correspond with actual increases and decreases in their self-esteem scale scores ( .56). Overall, the findings support the perspective that self-esteem, like other personality characteristics, can change in systematic ways while exhibiting continuity over time. Keywords: self-esteem, self-perceived change, emerging adulthood, college transition Global self-esteem refers to an individual’s subjective evaluation of his or her worth as a person. Like other personality characteristics, global self-esteem is moderately stable over time and across contexts (Trzesniewski, Donnellan, & Robins, 2003). This stable component of self-esteem has been a major research focus and has been shown to promote behaviors, goals, and coping mechanisms that facilitate success in work, school, and relationships and reduce risk for mental and physical health problems, substance abuse, and antisocial behavior (e.g., Crocker & Luhtanen, 2003; Donnellan, Trzesniewski, Robins, Moffit, & Caspi, 2005; Orth, Robins, & Widaman, 2012; Swann, ChangSchneider, & Larsen McClarty, 2007; Trzesniewski et al., 2006). In light of these findings, a better understanding of the way self-esteem changes during critical life periods, such as the college years, is an important societal goal. Although much research conducted to date has emphasized the stability of self-esteem, self-esteem is assumed to be dynamic and responsive to events occurring in one’s life. Emerging adulthood (age 18 years to mid-20s) is a period of considerable opportunity and challenge. Many young adults move away from home for the first time, begin college and full-time jobs, or marry and have children. Researchers interested in personality and development have emphasized the importance of this period, describing the complex challenges that young adults face and the patterns of adaptation that follow from their resolution (Arnett, 2011; Erikson, 1964; White, 1966). Given the transitional nature of these years, emerging adulthood may be a time during which global selfesteem is especially likely to change. Moreover, the developmental process of becoming an adult often entails a questioning of one’s identity and subsequent reformulation of conceptions and evaluations of the self. The present study examined consistency and change in selfesteem during emerging adulthood. As in most research on personality development, we examined the rank-order stability of self-esteem as well as mean-level (i.e., normative) change. We also examined predictors of the self-esteem trajectory, attempting to predict who increased or decreased in self-esteem as they progressed through college. We focused on one of the most significant indicators of success in the college context—academic achieveJoanne M. Chung and Richard W. Robins, Department of Psychology, University of California, Davis; Kali H. Trzesniewski, Department of Human Development, University of California, Davis; Erik E. Noftle, Department of Psychology, Willamette University; Brent W. Roberts, Department of Psychology, University of Illinois, Urbana–Champaign; Keith F. Widaman, Department of Psychology, University of California, Davis. This research was supported by National Institute of Mental Health Predoctoral Fellowship T32-MH2006 to Joanne M. Chung and National Institute on Drug Abuse Grant R01-DA-017902 to Richard W. Robins. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Joanne M. Chung, Department of Psychology, University of California, One Shields Avenue, Davis, CA 95616. E-mail: jmhchung@ucdavis.edu This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology © 2013 American Psychological Association 2013, Vol. 106, No. 1, 000 0022-3514/13/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0035135 1
CHUNG ET AL prisingly litle xmining mean-level change in self steem and whether individuals who enter college expecting to dually inere s durine the transition from eet their we exam d individuals sof how their self 2013).Hows ver.relatively little research has specifically charted in self-esteem ve review previous research on these topics. ignificant mean-level changes have not been found (va e Rank-Order Stability of Self-Esteem During College decline (Pritchard.Wilson.&Yamnitz.2007:Shim Dver the past few decade o which selfesteem should be 012) ptualized as a trait-like These latter at th ruct that er tim or as a stat out the study suggests that emere adults acros this difficul C2012. find high ne self-este n to rde bility:that individuals who】 rela deeline.plateau at a lower level than w hen they entere the sed by the relation be ween pers ality scores across twe e models in structural quation mode s over the entirety of the colle .Rank-order st nd then in over the 201 that change ur in spi wski et al's(2003)meta-an the rank-order stability of 2000 self for a hypo es in the 50s over the 4-vear period examined in the presen tud period even Mean-Level Changes in Self-Esteem During College age-de dent ta ons ng ho ons in self-esteem reflect changes artner)c (.g. death of c riend famil in old age.When these changes ar s th tran time.Mean-level hang iso sition to adoles which typically coin he t junior hi ch scho elf-esteem drop g.Eccles.Vigfield.a Mill ,1989 ersely lack of mean-level change does not indicate high stability Eccles.Wigfield.Midgley.&Reuman 1989:S1m the te m"ale changes i ot be reflected in aggregate mean-level change (e..if the nur s in RSE of people who decrease offsets the number of people who find ngs ca the
ment. Specifically, we tested whether individuals who receive good (poor) grades in college tend to increase (decrease) in selfesteem, and whether individuals who enter college expecting to receive very high grades maintain their self-esteem, or experience a drop in self-esteem when they fail to meet their expectations. Finally, we examined individuals’ perceptions of how their selfesteem changed during college and the degree to which these perceptions correspond to actual changes in self-esteem.1 Below, we review previous research on these topics. Rank-Order Stability of Self-Esteem During College Over the past few decades, researchers have debated the degree to which self-esteem should be conceptualized as a trait-like construct that remains relatively stable over time, or as a state-like process that continually fluctuates in response to environmental and situational stimuli (Donnellan, Kenny, Trzesniewski, Lucas, & Conger, 2012; Kuster & Orth, 2013; Trzesniewski et al., 2003). If self-esteem is trait-like, then we would expect to find high rankorder stability; that is, individuals who have relatively high (or low) self-esteem at one point in time will tend to have high (or low) self-esteem years later. Rank-order stability is commonly assessed by the correlation between personality scores across two time points, but it can also be evaluated with first-order autoregressive models in structural equation modeling. Rank-order stability is reduced by maturational or experiential factors that differentially affect people’s self-esteem, as well as by measurement error. The rank-order stability of self-esteem varies across the lifespan. Specifically, stability is relatively low during early childhood and increases throughout adolescence and adulthood (Donnellan et al., 2012; Kuster & Orth, 2013; Trzesniewski et al., 2003). In Trzesniewski et al.’s (2003) meta-analysis, the rank-order stability of self-esteem over a 4-year period for a hypothetical sample of 18-year-olds was .55. Accordingly, we expected to find test–retest estimates in the .50s over the 4-year period examined in the present study. Mean-Level Changes in Self-Esteem During College As individuals go through life, their self-esteem inevitably waxes and wanes. These fluctuations in self-esteem reflect changes in our social environment as well as maturational changes such as puberty and cognitive declines in old age. When these changes are normative, age-dependent, and affect individuals in a similar manner, they will lead to aggregate (or mean-level) changes in selfesteem over time. Mean-level change is both theoretically and statistically distinct from rank-order stability. Considerable meanlevel change does not indicate low rank-order stability, and conversely lack of mean-level change does not indicate high stability. For example, a group of people might increase substantially on a trait, but their rank ordering would stay the same if everyone in the group increased by the same amount. In the same way, the rank ordering of individuals could change substantially over time but not be reflected in aggregate mean-level change (e.g., if the number of people who decrease offsets the number of people who increase). Although we know that self-esteem shows normative changes across the lifespan (Robins & Trzesniewski, 2005), there is surprisingly little research examining mean-level change in selfesteem during the critical college period. Recent research suggests that self-esteem gradually increases during the transition from adolescence into adulthood (Erol & Orth, 2011; Orth, Trzesniewski, & Robins, 2010; Wagner, Lüdtke, Jonkmann, & Trautwein, 2013). However, relatively little research has specifically charted changes in self-esteem from the beginning to the end of college. When self-esteem has been assessed at the beginning and end of college, significant mean-level changes have not been found (van der Velde, Feij, & Taris, 1995). In contrast, two studies that examined self-esteem during the first year of college found a significant decline (Pritchard, Wilson, & Yamnitz, 2007; Shim, Ryan, & Cassady, 2012). These latter studies suggest that the transition to college may challenge emerging adults’ self-views, but the former study suggests that emerging adults are able to maintain their self-esteem across this difficult transition and throughout college. Taken together, these studies suggest a need to examine self-esteem across college in addition to during the transition to college. Such a study could provide insight into what happens after the initial self-esteem drop— do students continue to decline, plateau at a lower level than when they entered college, or show a recovery to their initial level. The present study aims to provide a more precise picture of the specific, year-by-year changes over the entirety of the college experience. Studies of self-esteem development across the lifespan have found that self-esteem decreases during the transition to adolescence and then increases gradually over the course of adulthood, before declining in old age (Erol & Orth, 2011; Orth et al., 2012; Robins, Tracy, Trzesniewski, Potter, & Gosling, 2001). These studies suggest that change can occur in response to transitions or major life events (e.g., Caspi & Roberts, 2001), although maturational changes such as hormonal changes in puberty (Williams & Currie, 2000) may also contribute to self-esteem change. For example, Rutter (1996) stated that, although there is stability in many types of behavior from childhood through adulthood, turning points modify or redirect life trajectories; turning points are transitional periods or events that have the potential to alter behavior, affect, cognition, or context, all of which could result in lifelong change. Normative, age-dependent transitions (e.g., leaving home for college) might explain normative change, whereas transitions that are not age dependent (e.g., breaking up with a romantic partner) or nonnormative (e.g., death of close friend or family member) may produce individual differences in change. The most frequently studied turning point for self-esteem development is the transition to adolescence, which typically coincides with the transition to junior high school. This school transition has been implicated as a major cause of the adolescent self-esteem drop (e.g., Eccles, Wigfield, Flanagan, & Miller, 1989; Eccles, Wigfield, Midgley, & Reuman, 1993; Marsh, 1989; Sim- 1 We use the term “actual” because we examined actual changes in a person’s self-esteem scale scores. We are not necessarily equating these changes in RSE scores with “real” changes in self-esteem. However, if one accepts scales such as the RSE as valid measures of self-esteem, then the present findings can be conceptualized as contrasting perceived and real changes in self-esteem. Indeed, in the literature, self-esteem is most commonly measured using standardized self-report scales, and therefore we are assessing (albeit imperfectly) actual change as it is represented in the current tradition of self-esteem assessment. This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. 2 CHUNG ET AL
SELF-ESTEEM DURING EMERGING ADULTHOOD whether changes in academic achievement are associated with 4 years 1987 beca children go through hange.Weknow of nore rch that directly addre r h heir s elf-esteem will be negatively i ted wher fail to mo Because the transition to college involves changes in schoo ir un other at hi expectation may parallel the transition to junior high school.Stuc s of the for grade Thus.given the tendency for n cordingly,we believe that the transition to college long s research has s found little or no change in mean levels of ple who think the will receive better grades than th he college years ntrollin g for actual grad Academic Achievement and Self-Esteem expectations about th r grades show in of the most salient features of the transition from high f residualized d in th self-enha ent liter y im lash with t Perceived Change in Self-Esteem (Marsh Hat 20031.Give .o published studies have ind esteem has changed to actu report large changes in the their college des and the actual grades the cople hat sc and Hattie's (1982)meta-analysis (e.g druff. 19g3 Woodruff&Birren.1972).The discrepa ooint average [GPA].test scores,etc. was 21.In iggests that not be ate a tine.DuB tend to d their sel n Ross resp (st rdized beta we Addit and act ality chan studi es have that c elated For exampl lohal sel previous arch has found that higher levels of e (Costa McCrac,1989: Costa. selr-hcleaiticularlyd oins et a academic achievement.However.these studies have not examined between perceived and actual change in the Big Five during
mons, Blyth, Van Cleave, & Bush, 1979; Wigfield & Eccles, 1994). However, the findings regarding this claim are somewhat inconsistent (Blyth, Simmons, & Bush, 1978; Hirsch & Rapkin, 1987; Nottleman, 1987), perhaps because children go through puberty and transition to middle and junior high school at different ages. Thus, a more homogenous transition such as the transition into college (where maturational changes may be less likely to play a role) is needed for studying the impact of a potentially stressful life transition on self-esteem development. Because the transition to college involves changes in school environment, social ties, expectations from others, and expectations for oneself, self-esteem change during the transition to college may parallel the transition to junior high school. Studies of the transition to adolescence suggest that the experience of changing school contexts can result in an immediate negative impact on self-esteem. Accordingly, we believe that the transition to college constitutes a developmental turning point that, on average, negatively impacts the self-esteem of young adults. In summary, previous research has found little or no change in mean levels of self-esteem from the beginning to the end of college. However, these studies have not examined change periodically throughout the college years. Academic Achievement and Self-Esteem One of the most salient features of the transition from high school to college is the increasing level of academic rigor and competitiveness that most students experience. This has been described as moving from being a “big fish in a little pond” to being a “little fish in a big pond.” This transition has been shown to negatively impact self-esteem as students’ high initial expectations clash with the reality of competing with other “big fish” for good grades, and the inevitable failure that follows for many of them (Marsh & Hau, 2003). Given the centrality of academic achievement in the college context, we focused on how the selfesteem trajectory is shaped by students’ initial expectations for their college grades and the actual grades they end up receiving. In general, the research literature suggests that school grades are positively associated with high self-esteem. For example, in Hansford and Hattie’s (1982) meta-analysis, the estimated effect size between various self-beliefs (e.g., self-esteem, self-acceptance, academic self-concept, etc.) and various academic outcomes (e.g., grade-point average [GPA], test scores, etc.) was .21. In a more recent meta-analysis, researchers examined the prospective influence of self-beliefs on academic achievement, in student samples that spanned pre-school to college (Valentine, DuBois, & Cooper, 2004). Results of this meta-analysis indicated that self-beliefs positively predicted later academic achievement, with an estimated effect size (standardized Beta weight) of .08. Additionally, Marsh and Craven (2006) provided evidence for reciprocal influence: Self-beliefs in the academic domain positively predicted later academic achievement, and, conversely, academic achievement positively predicted later self-beliefs; however, these effects held only for academic self-concept, not global self-esteem. In summary, previous research has found that higher levels of selfesteem, particularly domain-specific self-esteem (i.e., academic self-beliefs), are concurrently and longitudinally associated with academic achievement. However, these studies have not examined whether changes in academic achievement are associated with changes in self-esteem across 4 years of college. We also examined the influence of expected grades on selfesteem change. We know of no research that directly addresses this question, but previous research has consistently shown that college students have inflated expectations about their grades (Beyer, 1999; Garavalia & Gredler, 2002; Nowell & Alston, 2007; Svanum & Aigner, 2011; Wendorf, 2002), raising the possibility that self-esteem will be negatively impacted when students fail to meet their unrealistic expectations. Another related line of research shows that highly narcissistic students are more likely to expect high grades than less narcissistic students, independent of their actual ability (Farwell & Wohlwend-Lloyd, 1998), suggesting that overly inflated self-views are associated with inflated expectations for grades. Thus, given the tendency for narcissistic individuals to report high self-esteem, we expect to find a link between high expected grades and higher levels of self-esteem. However, over the long term, as narcissistic illusions about getting straight A’s confront the objective reality of academic mediocrity, we expect that people who think they will receive better grades than they actually receive will show declines in self-esteem over time. To address this question, we examined the effects of expected grades on self-esteem change, controlling for actual grades. By partialing actual grades out of expected grades, we are taking “reality” out of their expectations and examining whether people who have unrealistically positive expectations about their grades show increases or decreases in self-esteem over time (this is a variant on the use of residualized discrepancy scores in the self-enhancement literature; Kwan, John, Kenny, Bond, & Robins, 2004; Schriber & Robins, 2012). Perceived Change in Self-Esteem To our knowledge, no published studies have compared individuals’ perceptions of how their self-esteem has changed to actual change in their self-esteem scores. However, related research suggests that people retrospectively report large changes in their personality, despite showing relatively minor changes in their personality test scores (Fleeson & Heckhausen, 1997; Haslam, Fox, & Whelan, 2007; Robins, Noftle, Trzesniewski, & Roberts, 2005; Staudinger, Bluck, & Herzberg, 2003; Wilson & Ross, 2001; Woodruff, 1983; Woodruff & Birren, 1972). The discrepancy between folk notions of substantial and often dramatic personality change and empirical findings indicating modest, gradual personality change suggests that people may not be very accurate at describing their own personality development. These findings are consistent with theorizing that people tend to derogate past selves in order to make their current self feel better (Wilson & Ross, 2001), a process that would tend to decrease the correspondence between perceived and actual personality change. The few studies have that correlated individual differences in perceived personality change with actual changes in personality have produced mixed results. For example, Costa, McCrae, and colleagues concluded from their research that people’s perceptions of how their Big Five scores have changed over time are largely inaccurate (Costa & McCrae, 1989; Herbst, McCrae, Costa, Feaganes, & Siegler, 2000). In contrast, Robins et al. (2005) pointed to modest but statistically significant correlations (r .15 to .33) between perceived and actual change in the Big Five during This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. SELF-ESTEEM DURING EMERGING ADULTHOOD 3
CHUNG ET AL college o that people have at least some insight into the way The literature on perceived change in personality suggests that s at the first and last ents (first week and fourth miht not be were n and we might expect to find the stronge 05)and one historyand ntal phas (as evic hold incomes belov Tr et al.. rom fan es with household 100.00 change Present Study Measures nicity,sex and socioeconomic s (SESed their cth and ve group.Sex was coded as(male)and 1 (female).Socio about sel SES)to 5 (high SES).The ES ma diffe tlevel of education npleted.father's highest level of t is the traie of self erg.1965)scale thrce times during th are there men-eveordreasesnsf (RSE:R 80 to 01- sbetween expected and actualg ) m change?F th. and respond to actual changes GPA on30001400 Method scale)were obtained from university rco ds every semester for d Sample and Procedure (h ge.participants were This research used data from the Berkeley Longitudinal Stud ongitudinal to examine th bout the study. k you will attain at UC Berkeley "Wha first year of college and then assessed annually throughout college ermade on a GPA scale ranging(ll)to00 Perceived self-esteem change.Perce was as ssed at the end the first (N=306).second (N=260).third (N=200).and fourth yeof Rosenberg Self-Estcem Scale eek of college(N 489)end of the first er (N 05 and end of the first ().second ().third (N197). stud山y
college to argue that people have at least some insight into the way their personality changes over time. The literature on perceived change in personality suggests that there might not be a strong correspondence between perceived and actual change in self-esteem. However, of all the developmental stages, we might expect to find the strongest correspondence during emerging adulthood, because young adults are actively focused on identity issues (Erikson, 1964; Harter, 1999; Pals, 1999) and engaging in a process of “open exploration” (Fleeson & Heckhausen, 1997, p. 134). Constructing one’s identity necessitates a good deal of self-reflection, which may promote insight into one’s life history and experiences. Moreover, there is a substantial amount of intraindividual variability in changes in self-esteem during this developmental phase (as evidenced by relatively low rank-order stability; Trzesniewski et al., 2003), which should increase the range of individual differences in self-esteem, making it easier to find stronger correlations between perceived and actual change. Present Study The present study examined self-esteem continuity and change in emerging adulthood using longitudinal data on a large sample of young adults followed through their college years. Participants completed the Rosenberg (1965) Self-Esteem scale six times over the course of college. These data provide a unique opportunity to learn more about self-esteem change during an important developmental transition. More specifically, the present research addresses four main questions concerning self-esteem development in emerging adulthood: First, to what extent are individual differences in self-esteem stable through the college years? Second, what is the trajectory of self-esteem over the course of college; that is, are there mean-level increases or decreases in self-esteem during this time? Third, to what extent do changes in grades and discrepancies between expected and actual grades predict selfesteem change? Fourth, to what extent do young adults think their self-esteem has changed, and how well do these perceptions correspond to actual changes in self-esteem scores? Method Sample and Procedure This research used data from the Berkeley Longitudinal Study, a longitudinal study designed to examine the development of self-esteem and personality during college (for further details about the study, see Noftle & Robins, 2007; Robins & Beer, 2001; Robins, Fraley, Roberts, & Trzesniewski, 2001; Robins et al., 2005).2 Participants were recruited during the first week of their first year of college and then assessed annually throughout college. Participants were contacted by mail and asked to complete an extensive questionnaire in exchange for financial compensation. Six assessments were conducted over a 4-year period: first week of college (N 508); end of the first semester (N 455); and end of the first (N 306), second (N 260), third (N 200), and fourth (N 303) years of college. The Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (Rosenberg, 1965) was administered at every assessment: first week of college (N 489); end of the first semester (N 405); and end of the first (N 306), second (N 295), third (N 197), and fourth (N 303) years of college. Our analyses focused on a subsample of participants (N 295) who completed the relevant measures at the first and last assessments (first week and fourth year). On most variables of interest, there were no differences between participants and nonparticipants. However, participants received significantly higher grades throughout college than nonparticipants (Mdn .24 grade points across the eight assessments, ps .05) and were more likely to be women (60% vs. 50%, p .05). In light of these differences, the participants in the study may not represent an unbiased sample of the original study participants. The sample was diverse in terms of ethnicity (43% Asian, 36% Caucasian, 12% Chicano/Latino, 5% African American, 4% Missing/Other/Multiracial), sex (60% female), socioeconomic status (20% came from families with 1992 household incomes below $25,000 and 17% from families with household incomes above $100,000), and academic ability (combined verbal and math SAT scores ranged from 650 to 1,530; M 1,194, SD 173). Measures Demographic variables. Participants self-reported their ethnicity, sex, and socioeconomic status (SES; see Table 1 for correlations among demographic variables). To retain adequate sample sizes, we classified participants as Caucasian, Asian, or non-Asian minority; this variable was dummy coded with Caucasians as the reference group. Sex was coded as 0 (male) and 1 (female). Socioeconomic status (SES) ranged from 1 (low SES) to 5 (high SES). The SES variable is a composite of three items that assessed mother’s highest level of education completed, father’s highest level of education completed, and annual family income. Self-esteem. Participants completed the 10-item Rosenberg Self-Esteem (RSE; Rosenberg, 1965) scale three times during the first year of college, and then annually for the next 3 years (alpha reliability ranged from .89 to .91; see Table 1 for all estimates). Items were rated on a 5-point scale ranging from 1 (not very true of me) to 5 (very true of me). To examine change in self-esteem, we conducted a latent growth curve model (LGCM; Bollen & Curran, 2006) using all six RSE assessments. Academic achievement. Student GPAs (on a 0.00 to 4.00 scale) were obtained from university records every semester for 4 years (eight assessments total). Expected academic achievement. During the first week of college, participants were asked three questions about their expected performance in college: “What overall GPA do you think you are capable of attaining at UC Berkeley?” “Realistically, what overall GPA do you think you will attain at UC Berkeley?” “What is the lowest overall GPA you would be satisfied attaining at UC Berkeley?” (alpha reliability .87). For all three items, responses were made on a GPA scale ranging from 0.00 (all F’s) to 4.00 (all A’s). Perceived self-esteem change. Perceived self-esteem change was assessed at the end of the fourth year of college. Participants were 2 The following articles also report self-esteem findings using data from the Berkeley Longitudinal Study: Orth, Robins, and Meier (2009); Orth, Robins, and Roberts (2008); Robins and Beer (2001); Robins, Hendin, and Trzesniewski (2001); Robins and Pals (2002). However, the research questions addressed by these studies overlap only minimally with the present study. This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. 4 CHUNG ET AL
SELF-ESTEEM DURING EMERGING ADULTHOOD Table 1 Intercorrelations Among Study Variables Variable 2 8 10 11 12 13 a status GP ek D change 1006000 Cohen'sd 30 ote GPA grade-point av below the diagonal.Valueson the diagonal in parentheses are apha reliability nts are above th PA is the GPA as xpected Cohen'sds are reported for differences c05. asked to rate how their"self-esteem.self-confidence"has changed autoregressive model to the six assessments.First-order autore schas demonstrad that singl-item a statistical nuisance.first-order autoregressive models take lam,&Jans,201:Robins,Fraley.et al2001). order autoregressive models provide estimate Results of the latent Rank-Order Stability of Self-Esteem first-order autoregressive model that allowed for the estimatio f measu ment error. ixing the urement eror to be equa ank ord Rank-order stability for self-esteem was consistently high,with 3.81 49 4 密密. el of sel
asked to rate how their “self-esteem, self-confidence” has changed since they entered college. The rating scale ranged from 1 (decreased) to 5 (increased), with the midpoint labeled stayed the same. Past research has demonstrated that single-item measures, such as our measure of perceived self-esteem change, can have adequate reliability and validity (Gosling, Rentfrow, & Swann, 2003; Postmes, Haslam, & Jans, 2012; Robins, Fraley, et al., 2001). Results Rank-Order Stability of Self-Esteem To examine the rank-order stability of self-esteem during college, we used structural equation modeling to fit a first-order autoregressive model to the six assessments. First-order autoregressive models are often used to examine repeated measures data. Whereas traditional OLS methods treat autocorrelation as a statistical nuisance, first-order autoregressive models take into account the dependency of the previous assessment on subsequent assessments so that each assessment is composed of a random error component and the previous assessment. Firstorder autoregressive models provide estimates of the latent variable separately from error, allowing for stability estimates that have been corrected for reliability of the measure. We fit a first-order autoregressive model that allowed for the estimation of measurement error, fixing the measurement error to be equal across assessments (see Figure 1 for model and estimates). Rank-order stability for self-esteem was consistently high, with Table 1 Intercorrelations Among Study Variables Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 1. Asiana — 2. Non-Asian minoritya .43 — 3. Femaleb .04 .00 — 4. Socioeconomic status .00 .36 .05 — 5. College GPAc .01 .22 .15 .31 — 6. Expected college GPAd .15 .19 .17 .12 .14 (.87) 7. Self-esteem (Week 1) .12 .01 .10 .11 .05 .11 (.89) .89 .83 .77 .69 .64 8. Self-esteem (Semester 1) .10 .06 .10 .08 .04 .11 .79 (.89) .86 .78 .63 .62 9. Self-esteem (Year 1) .14 .08 .20 .11 .05 .10 .77 .79 (.91) .87 .73 .67 10. Self-esteem (Year 2) .16 .12 .14 .10 .24 .03 .70 .72 .79 (.90) .88 .77 11. Self-esteem (Year 3) .17 .06 .10 .02 .04 .03 .63 .60 .66 .81 (.90) .83 12. Self-esteem (Year 4) .16 .00 .03 .05 .18 .01 .58 .56 .60 .70 .78 (.91) 13. Perceived self-esteem change .06 .05 .12 .02 .07 .02 .06 .13 .18 .18 .36 .32 — M .45 .18 .60 3.52 3.20 3.43 3.81 3.51 3.85 3.90 3.89 3.92 3.81 SD .50 .39 .49 1.00 .49 .30 .75 .63 .79 .79 .76 .74 1.09 Cohen’s de .68 .72 .14 .19 .20 Note. GPA grade-point average. Raw correlation coefficients are below the diagonal. Values on the diagonal in parentheses are alpha reliability coefficients. Disattenuated coefficients are above the diagonal. a Asian and non-Asian minority variables are dummy coded so that the reference group is European American. b Female is dummy coded so that the reference group is male. c College GPA is the GPA assessment at the end of the fourth year of college. d Expected College GPA is the mean of the three expected GPA variables assessed at Week 1. e Cohen’s ds are reported for differences between adjacent assessments. Cohen’s d between the Week 1 and Year 4 assessment .16. p .05. Figure 1. First-order autoregressive model of self-esteem stability across 4 years of college. Unstandardized estimates are presented in the figure. Standardized stability estimates are presented in parentheses. All estimates are significant at p .05. The “S” denotes a latent status variable, which is indicated by the corresponding manifest variable (in rectangles). This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. SELF-ESTEEM DURING EMERGING ADULTHOOD 5
CHUNG ET AL =.90.Sce cond.ss ofen amined isueis whether the amount of sent the large decrease in self-esteem that y self-este riance of adaS s iteration of this model,th dress this issue.we first fit a fullyun strained model to the six negative,representing a Hey od case.However.the residua a model in which all do llop cept.the RMSEA)- .157,9%confidence index(TL)900.To tive fit inc 6 ces to be qual Th mod .000. dfit to the data.(4) =61,RM SEA 00,.90 .061.CFI he first sem ended to increase in self-esteem over the 4 vears of colle Th Mean-Level Change in Self-Esteem Despite this variability,the de inself-esteer norm urve moc ing (LGC al lor exa mining ed variables are repre cept endy.wes Inth slop nd slon ariables.the intercept was negatively lege e than those who The 375 and .750 vears after the first ass ent.respec ively.The higher people'ss-esteem.the le ents-at the end of the second tha the "end of first year"assessment.so were given coefficients of We next fit a conditional model (Model 1)to examine the potent 3. our onditiona model yielded good fit ndicati hat rem started 34 SES.The effects of demo hic variables on the first Ticant indicating 33 Wock I Semester I Year 1 Year 2 Year 3 Year 4 similar across sex.race.and SES (See Table 2 for esti Figure 2.Mean-level of self-esteem at each assessment. mates)
standardized estimates ranging from .89 to .95, Mdn .90. See Table 1 for stability correlations between all assessments. A second, less often examined issue is whether the amount of individual variability in self-esteem (i.e., the variance of selfesteem scores) changed systematically across assessments. To address this issue, we first fit a fully unconstrained model to the six self-esteem assessments in which we freely estimated means, variances, and covariances of the manifest variables. Variance estimates were .56 (Week 1), .38 (Semester 1), .61 (Year 1), .61 (Year 2), .55 (Year 3), and .55 (Year 4). Not surprisingly, given the comparatively low Semester 1 variance, a model in which all variances were constrained to be equal did not fit the data well, 2 (5) 41.452, p .00, root-mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA) .157, 90% confidence interval (CI) [.115, .203], pclose fit .000, comparative fit index (CFI) .967, Tucker–Lewis index (TLI) .900. To improve model fit, we allowed the variance for Semester 1 to be freely estimated, but constrained all other variances to be equal. This model fit significantly better than the fully constrained model, change 2 (1) 38.737, p .000, and had good fit to the data, 2 (4) 2.715, p .61, RMSEA .000, 90% CI [.000, .074], pclose fit .843, CFI 1.000, TLI 1.004 Thus, the first semester of college seems to have reduced individual variability in self-esteem, but the amount of variance returned to initial levels at all subsequent time points. Mean-Level Change in Self-Esteem Figure 2 shows mean levels of self-esteem at each assessment. The trajectory of self-esteem was examined using latent growth curve modeling (LGCM), which is ideal for examining meanlevel change as well as individual differences in change. In an LGCM, observed variables are represented by a latent intercept, reflecting initial levels of the variable, and a latent slope, representing growth in this variable over time. In the present study, we specified a modified LGCM, given the unique pattern shown at Semester 1 and the uneven spacing of assessments. The slope was centered at the first time of measurement, so this coefficient was 0. The two remaining assessments in the first year were at the end of first semester and end of first year, or .375 and .750 years after the first assessment, respectively. The remaining assessments—at the end of the second, third, and fourth years of college—were annual assessments subsequent to the “end of first year” assessment, so were given coefficients of 1.75, 2.75, and 3.75, respectively. In addition to the latent intercept and latent slope variables, we added a single-indicator latent variable to represent the large decrease in self-esteem that occurred at Semester 1. In a previous iteration of this model, the residual variance estimate of the Semester 1 assessment was negative, representing a Heywood case. However, the residual variance of the Semester 1 assessment was not significantly different from 0, and consequently, the Semester 1 assessment was fixed at 0 to allow for more appropriate estimates. For all other assessments, residual variance was estimated to be equal. Below we refer to the latent intercept as the self-esteem intercept, the single-indicator latent variable as the 1st semester slope, and the latent slope as the 4-year slope. These variables were all allowed to covary with each other. We first fit an unconditional model (Model 0) to determine whether there was an average, or typical, trajectory of self-esteem throughout the college years (see Figure 3). This model yielded good fit, 2 (17, N 295) 44.788, RMSEA .074, 90% CI [.048, .101], pclosefit .061, CFI .974, TLI .977. The means of the intercept, first semester slope, and 4-year slope all differed significantly from zero, indicating that people started out at an average level of self-esteem, that people tended to decrease in self-esteem during the first semester of college, and that people tended to increase in self-esteem over the 4 years of college. The variance of each of the three latent variables was also significant, suggesting that there was individual variability around the average trajectory. Despite this variability, the decline in self-esteem occurring during the first semester was highly normative, with the variability in the slope largely reflecting differences in the degree to which people declined in self-esteem, not whether they exhibited a decline. In terms of the correlations among the intercept and slope variables, the intercept was negatively associated with the firstsemester slope, indicating that people who entered college with higher self-esteem tended to show less steep declines in selfesteem during their first semester of college than those who entered college with lower self-esteem. The intercept was negatively associated with the 4-year slope, indicating that the higher people’s self-esteem, the less they increased in selfesteem across college. However, the 1st semester slope was not significantly associated with the 4-year slope, indicating that the decrease in self-esteem occurring during the first semester of college was not associated with overall change in self-esteem throughout college. We next fit a conditional model (Model 1) to examine the potential moderating effects of our demographic variables, entering participant sex, race, and SES as time-invariant covariates in our model. The conditional model yielded good fit, 2 (29, N 295) 60.049, RMSEA .060, 90% CI [.038, .082], pclosefit .202, CFI .974, TLI .960. Significant effects of participant sex and SES were found on the intercept, indicating that females and those of lower SES started out with lower self-esteem in college than males and those of higher SES. The effects of demographic variables on the first semester slope and 4-year slope variables were nonsignificant, indicating that the trajectory of self-esteem throughout the college years was similar across sex, race, and SES (See Table 2 for estiFigure 2. Mean-level of self-esteem at each assessment. mates). This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. 6 CHUNG ET AL
SELF-ESTEEM DURING EMERGING ADULTHOOD 8受奥兴 752.75 3.75 375 -08,36y .01(-.14 -03(-39° 3.81 03 ent slope to e Year 375 ted in the figun are in parenthes s.In an e of th s fixed mo p<05 raject ory From Actual receive poor grades tend to decline in selfe teem?To were ind atent difference score analysis(MeArdle) s and the paths then rela ved scoreisde mposed into a latent error.The score.An intercept was specified by creating a path to the Asian Socioccono SEB 5E& p<05
Predicting the Self-Esteem Trajectory From Actual and Expected Academic Achievement Do individuals who receive good grades show increases in self-esteem as they go through college, and conversely do those who receive poor grades tend to decline in self-esteem? To address this question, we modeled changes in GPA using an latent difference score analysis (McArdle & Hamagami, 2001) and then related the slope and intercept from this model to the self-esteem trajectory (Model 2). In LDS analysis, each observed score is decomposed into a latent variable and error. The change in this latent variable between two points of time is defined as a latent difference score. LDS analysis is optimal for a variable such as cumulative GPA because it includes an autoregressive component that explicitly models dependence of a score at one assessment with a score at a prior assessment. In the current study, each of the eight GPA assessments was indicated by a latent status variable. Latent difference scores were indicated by a variable with paths from previous status and to current status. To allow for estimation of the latent difference scores, the paths between all status variables and the paths between the latent difference score variable with current status were fixed at unity. Variances were estimated for each difference score. An intercept was specified by creating a path to the initial status variable, and a slope was specified by creating paths to all seven of the difference scores. Figure 3. Self-esteem trajectory (Model 0—no covariates). Because the Semester 1 assessment occurred about three and a half months after Week 1, the path from latent slope to Semester 1 was fixed at .375. The path from latent slope to the Year 1 assessment was fixed at .75 to represent another difference of three and a half months. Each subsequent assessment was collected yearly, and thus the path from latent slope to Year 2 assessment was fixed at 1.75; for the Year 3 assessment, fixed at 2.75; and for the Year 4 assessment, fixed at 3.75. Unstandardized estimates are presented in the figure. Standardized estimates are in parentheses. In a previous iteration of this model, the residual variance estimate of the Semester 1 assessment was negative, representing a Heywood case. However, the residual variance of the Semester 1 assessment was not significantly different from 0, and, consequently, the Semester 1 assessment was fixed at 0 to allow for more appropriate estimates. p .05. Table 2 Model 1: Effects of Demographic Covariates on Self-Esteem Trajectory Latent variable Demographic covariate Asian Non-Asian minority Female Socioeconomic status B SE B B SE B B B SE B B B SE B Self-esteem intercept .13 .10 .09 .14 .13 .08 .21 .09 .15 .10 .05 .14 Self-esteem slope: First semester .06 .06 .10 .07 .08 .09 .10 .05 .15 .04 .03 .11 Self-esteem slope: 4 years .04 .02 .16 .06 .03 .18 .04 .02 .14 .02 .01 .16 p .05. This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. SELF-ESTEEM DURING EMERGING ADULTHOOD 7
CHUNG ET AL 281,N=295)=163.636.RMSEA=050.90%CI[046.072 28,CF Expected GPA d the 4 lope.indicating that increa es in GPA were 二 VotPgrade-point average. this question we fit a which a latent variable indicated This reported that thei 97. ad h egatively associated ndic d ase in mean scores on the RSE.However, enuated increase.over the 4 years of college (see Table 4 for not coespond with changes in the actual Co examine whether expected GPA redicts self-esteem ived clt-esteen change as c of the the self This 066 211,C12 83.T sslopeoyocadour asting d in Moc icted the se traje ntly of actual GPA heir RSe scale Moreover,the em inte lege witl unr ated nge,sugg o the actually r decline in self-e lves even more self-esteemthe re rea h ie del 4a:see Figu did not atten over th not just the firs Perceived Change in Self-Esteem section.we examined cha res on the Discussion exan esteem in a sample of young men and women followed longi CollASEc GPA intercept GPA slope Variable B SEB BB SEB B ally receiv chg-e em change
The GPA intercept and slope were then allowed to covary with the self-esteem variables. This conditional model yielded good fit, 2 (81, N 295) 163.636, RMSEA .050, 90% CI [.046, .072], pclosefit .128, CFI .986, TLI .984. A significant association was found between the GPA slope and the 4-year self-esteem slope, indicating that increases in GPA were associated with increases in self-esteem (see Table 3 for estimates). We next examined whether individuals who enter college expecting to receive high grades maintain their self-esteem, or experience a drop in self-esteem when they fail to meet their expectations.3 To address this question, we fit a conditional model (Model 3) in which a latent variable indicated by the three expected GPA items was entered as a predictor of the self-esteem variables. This conditional model yielded good fit, 2 (32, N 295) 70.443, RMSEA .064, 90% CI [.044, .084], pclosefit .123, CFI .976, TLI .973. Expected GPA had a significant effect on the self-esteem intercept, indicating that students who entered college with high expectations for academic achievement tended to have high levels of self-esteem. Expected GPA was also negatively associated with the 4-year slope, indicating that students who entered college with high expectations for academic achievement experienced a drop in self-esteem, or at least an attenuated increase, over the 4 years of college (see Table 4 for estimates). To examine whether expected GPA predicts self-esteem independently of actual GPA, we next fit a conditional model (Model 4) in which both expected and actual GPA were entered as predictors of self-esteem. This conditional model yielded good fit, 2 (118, N 295) 224.123, RMSEA .055, 90% CI [.044, .066], pclosefit .211, CFI .983, TLI .981. All of the effects observed in Models 1 and 2 replicated in Model 3, and no new effects emerged (see Table 5 for estimates). Thus, expected GPA predicted the self-esteem trajectory independently of actual GPA. This suggests that individuals who entered college with unrealistically positive expectations about their grades— unrealistic relative to the grades they actually received— declined in self-esteem relative to those who entered college with more realistic appraisals of their academic future. Inclusion of the demographic covariates (Model 4a; see Figure 4) did not attenuate the significant effects of actual and expected GPA on the self-esteem intercept and selfesteem slope variables (see Table 5 for estimates). Perceived Change in Self-Esteem In the previous section, we examined changes in scores on the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale over a 4-year period. In this section, we examine participants’ subjective impressions of how they changed during college. 67% of our participants reported that their self-esteem had increased (score of 4 or 5), 21% reported that their self-esteem had stayed the same (score of 3), and only 12% reported that their self-esteem had declined (score of 1 or 2). Thus, the majority of participants believed their self-esteem improved during their 4 years in college, which is consistent with the observed increase in mean scores on the RSE. However, it is possible that these aggregate trends are consistent, yet at the individual level participants’ perceptions of how they changed do not correspond with changes in the actual scores. To examine the correspondence between perceived and actual change, we fit a conditional model (Model 5) where we entered perceived self-esteem change as a covariate of the self-esteem trajectory, regressing perceived self-esteem change onto the selfesteem intercept, first-semester slope, and 4-year slope (see Figure 5). The 4-year slope was positively associated with perceived changes in self-esteem (see Table 6), suggesting that individuals who thought they had increased (or decreased) in self-esteem did tend to actually increase (or decrease) in self-esteem, based on their RSE scale scores. Moreover, the self-esteem intercept was positively associated with perceived change, suggesting that individuals with higher initial levels of self-esteem tended to perceive themselves as gaining even more self-esteem as they progressed through college. The first-semester slope was not associated with perceived change, which is not surprising given that perceived change was assessed over the entire 4-year period, not just the first semester. The inclusion of demographic variables on perceived self-esteem change was not significant, indicating that the sex, race, and SES of the participant did not influence perceptions of self-esteem change (see Table 6 for coefficients).4 Discussion The present research examined stability and change in selfesteem in a sample of young men and women followed longi- 3 On average, the participants in our study “realistically” expected to attain a GPA of 3.43 (SD 0.33), yet the mean GPA at the end of the fourth year was 3.07 (SD 0.47), .36 grade points lower (d .90). Overall, 72% of participants expected to receive higher grades than they actually received. 4 One interesting question is whether grades are associated with perceived change in self-esteem. We fit a model correlating GPA with perceived self-esteem change and found that GPA is not associated with perceived change in self-esteem. Thus, based on our data, changes in GPA are only associated with changes in “actual” self-esteem, but not with perceived self-esteem change. Table 3 Model 2: Effects of College GPA on Self-Esteem-Trajectory Variable GPA intercept GPA slope B SE B B SE B Self-esteem intercept .02 .03 .05 .00 .01 .05 Self-esteem slope: First semester .02 .02 .09 .00 .00 .01 Self-esteem slope: 4 years .00 .01 .06 .00 .00 .17 Note. GPA grade-point average. p .05. Table 4 Model 3: Effects of Expected GPA on Self-Esteem-Trajectory Variable Expected GPA B SE B Self-esteem intercept .52 .19 .18 Self-esteem slope: First semester .06 .11 .05 Self-esteem slope: Four years .11 .05 .20 Note. GPA grade-point average. p .05. This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. 8 CHUNG ET AL.
SELF-ESTEEM DURING EMERGING ADULTHOOD Self-estem Self-esteem slo ope:4 year Predictor SE B SE B SE B 00.01 02/.02 2/01 1/.0 Asian minority SES 爱的心山 udinally over 4 years of college.Given the centrality of aca ture of the present is that oh e ceptions of change correspond with actual changes in thei ature on how self-esteemchane throughout the life course self-esteem trajectory.In addition,one particularly unique fea providing much needed data on the emerging adult period 261 24 24201个23201 .01(24 03(23y 05(74 0001) 01(12) 51.180 02(.10 1120 00(02) 04-37 (GPA (Model 4a).Th 17992. ers to the for GP you
tudinally over 4 years of college. Given the centrality of academic achievement to the college experience, we also examined how specific aspects of academic achievement, such as grades earned throughout college and expectations for grades at the beginning of college, predicted individual differences in this self-esteem trajectory. In addition, one particularly unique feature of the present research is that we examined both actual and perceived changes in self-esteem, and how strongly individuals’ perceptions of change correspond with actual changes in their self-esteem scores. Our findings contribute to a growing literature on how self-esteem changes throughout the life course by providing much needed data on the emerging adult period. Table 5 Models 4 and 4a: Effects of Actual and Expected GPA on Self-Esteem-Trajectory (With and Without Demographic Covariates) Predictor Self-esteem intercept Self-esteem slope: First semester Self-esteem slope: 4 years B SE B B SE B B B SE B GPA intercept .00/.01 .02/.02 .01/.08 .02/.01 .01/.01 .10/.09 .01/.01 .01/.01 .12/.08 GPA slope .00/.00 .01/.01 .01/.02 .00/.00 .00/.00 .02/.00 .00/.00 .00/.00 .23 /.18 Expected GPA .51/.50 .19/.19 .18 /.18 .06/.05 .11/.11 .05/.04 .11/.09 .05/.05 .20 /.17 Female —/.16 —/.09 —/.12 —/.09 —/.05 —/.15 —/.03 —/.02 —/.11 Asian —/.15 —/.10 —/.11 —/.07 —/.06 —/.11 —/.04 —/.02 —/.15 Non-Asian minority —/.18 —/.13 —/.10 —/.08 —/.08 —/.10 —/.07 —/.03 —/.20 SES —/.09 —/.05 —/.13 —/.03 —/.03 —/.11 —/.02 —/.01 —/.14 Notes. GPA grade-point average. Model 4 estimates (without demographic covariates) are on the left of the backslash, and Model 4a estimates (with demographic covariates) are on the right. Dashes denote that the variable was not entered into the model. p .05. Figure 4. Effects of actual and expected grade-point average (GPA) on self-esteem-trajectory (Model 4a). The unit constant and associated means are omitted from the figure for simplicity. Manifest variables for self-esteem trajectory are omitted from the figure. Coefficients for demographic variables are presented in Table 5. Unstandardized estimates are presented in the figure. Standardized estimates are in parentheses. “FA92” refers to the manifest variables for GPA at the end of fall semester in 1992, and “SP93” refers to the manifest variable for GPA at the end of spring semester in 1993, and so on. The “s” projecting on to these variables refers to the latent status variables for GPA, and the “d” refers to the latent difference scores for GPA. “exgpa1” refers to the manifest variable, “What overall GPA do you think you are capable of attaining at UC Berkeley?” “exgpa2” refers to the manifest variable, “Realistically, what overall GPA do you think you will attain at UC Berkeley?” “exgpa3” refers to the manifest variable, “What is the lowest overall GPA you would be satisfied attaining at UC Berkeley?” p .05. This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. SELF-ESTEEM DURING EMERGING ADULTHOOD 9
10 CHUNG ET AL 14 1.752.75 3.75 Self- 375 Esteem 4-year 1st 0 Slope 44 Intercept (09 Semeste .02 Slope -06(.-32)*2 2-.01(6.18) 203.38 3.641 -26* .10 .58(16) 4.85(.56) 1.32 .61(.38) Asian Non-Asian Perceived Self- Minority Female SES Esteem Change 88 p<05. Below we discuss the implications of the findings with regard Rank-Order Stability of Self-Esteem continuity in self-c rtime and thus Increa hat person ghe consistently high,indicating that people maintained their relative evels of stab self-esteem found in longitudinal studies of self-esteem and p strongly during the first semester. nality traits Mean-Level Change in Self-Esteem 1995).The high rank-orde stability of self-est
Below we discuss the implications of the findings with regard to our central research questions. Rank-Order Stability of Self-Esteem The current study sought to examine the stability of individual differences in self-esteem. Our results show that stability was consistently high, indicating that people maintained their relative positions within the sample; those who entered college with high levels of self-esteem tended to leave college with high levels of self-esteem, and vice versa. The stability coefficients from the current study are comparable to or higher than what has been found in longitudinal studies of self-esteem and personality traits during young adulthood (Roberts & DelVecchio, 2000; Robins, Fraley, et al., 2001; Trzesniewski et al., 2003; van der Velde et al., 1995). The high rank-order stability of self-esteem during this period suggests that, as individuals make the transition into adulthood, maturational changes are reduced, the sociocultural context is increasingly subject to individual control, and a more stable sense of self is formed. These processes would tend to promote continuity in self-concept over time and thus increase stability coefficients. These findings offer support for self-attitudes showing high levels of stability, which counters Conley’s (1984) claim that personality traits and intelligence have higher levels of stability than self-attitudes. Our results also show that the variability of individual differences was significantly restricted during the time that average self-esteem levels were at their lowest. This finding suggests that the college transition affected students similarly and strongly during the first semester. Mean-Level Change in Self-Esteem The current study also sought to examine the trajectory of self-esteem throughout the entire college experience, an important Figure 5. Correspondence between self-esteem trajectory and perceived self-esteem change (Model 5). Coefficients for demographic variables are presented in Table 6. Unstandardized estimates are presented in the figure. Standardized estimates are in parentheses. SES socioeconomic status. p .05. This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. 10 CHUNG ET AL