[This Article appeared in the American Scientist (Nov-Dec 1990),Volume 78,550-558.Retyped and posted with permission.] The Science of Scientific Writing If the reader is to grasp what the writer means, the writer must understand what the reader needs George D.Gopen and Judith A.Swan* *George D.Gopen is associate professor of English and Director of Writing Programs at Duke University.He holds a Ph.D. in English from Harvard University and a J.D.from Harvard Law School.Judith A.Swan teaches scientific writing at Princeton University.Her Ph.D..which is in biochemistry,was earned at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology.Address for Gopen:307 Allen Building,Duke University,Durham,NC 27706 Science is often hard to read.Most people assume that its difficulties are born out of necessity,out of the extreme complexity of scientific concepts,data and analysis.We argue here that complexity of thought need not lead to impenetrability of expression;we demonstrate a number of rhetorical principles that can produce clarity in communication without oversimplifying scientific issues.The results are substantive,not merely cosmetic:Improving the quality of writing actually improves the quality of thought The fundamental purpose of scientific discourse is not the mere presentation of information and thought, but rather its actual communication.It does not matter how pleased an author might be to have converted all the right data into sentences and paragraphs;it matters only whether a large majority of the reading audience accurately perceives what the author had in mind.Therefore,in order to understand how best to improve writing,we would do well to understand better how readers go about reading.Such an understanding has recently become available through work done in the fields of rhetoric,linguistics and cognitive psychology.It has helped to produce a methodology based on the concept of reader expectations. Writing with the Reader in Mind:Expectation and Context Readers do not simply read;they interpret.Any piece of prose,no matter how short,may "mean"in 10 (or more)different ways to 10 different readers.This methodology of reader expectations is founded on the recognition that readers make many of their most important interpretive decisions about the substance of prose based on clues they receive from its structure. This interplay between substance and structure can be demonstrated by something as basic as a simple
[This Article appeared in the American Scientist (Nov-Dec 1990), Volume 78, 550-558. Retyped and posted with permission.] The Science of Scientific Writing If the reader is to grasp what the writer means, the writer must understand what the reader needs George D. Gopen and Judith A. Swan* *George D. Gopen is associate professor of English and Director of Writing Programs at Duke University. He holds a Ph.D. in English from Harvard University and a J.D. from Harvard Law School. Judith A. Swan teaches scientific writing at Princeton University. Her Ph.D., which is in biochemistry, was earned at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology. Address for Gopen: 307 Allen Building, Duke University, Durham, NC 27706 Science is often hard to read. Most people assume that its difficulties are born out of necessity, out of the extreme complexity of scientific concepts, data and analysis. We argue here that complexity of thought need not lead to impenetrability of expression; we demonstrate a number of rhetorical principles that can produce clarity in communication without oversimplifying scientific issues. The results are substantive, not merely cosmetic: Improving the quality of writing actually improves the quality of thought. The fundamental purpose of scientific discourse is not the mere presentation of information and thought, but rather its actual communication. It does not matter how pleased an author might be to have converted all the right data into sentences and paragraphs; it matters only whether a large majority of the reading audience accurately perceives what the author had in mind. Therefore, in order to understand how best to improve writing, we would do well to understand better how readers go about reading. Such an understanding has recently become available through work done in the fields of rhetoric, linguistics and cognitive psychology. It has helped to produce a methodology based on the concept of reader expectations. Writing with the Reader in Mind: Expectation and Context Readers do not simply read; they interpret. Any piece of prose, no matter how short, may "mean" in 10 (or more) different ways to 10 different readers. This methodology of reader expectations is founded on the recognition that readers make many of their most important interpretive decisions about the substance of prose based on clues they receive from its structure. This interplay between substance and structure can be demonstrated by something as basic as a simple
table.Let us say that in tracking the temperature of a liquid over a period of time,an investigator takes measurements every three minutes and records a list of temperatures.Those data could be presented by a number of written structures.Here are two possibilities: t(time)=15',T(temperature)=32°,t=0',T=25°; t=6',T=29°;t=3',T=27°;t=12',T=32°;t=9'; T=31° time (min) temperature(°c) 0 25 3 27 6 29 9 31 32 15 32 Precisely the same information appears in both formats,yet most readers find the second easier to interpret.It may be that the very familiarity of the tabular structure makes it easier to use.But,more significantly,the structure of the second table provides the reader with an easily perceived context(time) in which the significant piece of information(temperature)can be interpreted.The contextual material appears on the left in a pattern that produces an expectation of regularity;the interesting results appear on the right in a less obvious pattern,the discovery of which is the point of the table. If the two sides of this simple table are reversed,it becomes much harder to read. temperature(°c) time (min) 25 0 27 3 29 6 31 9 32 12 32 15 Since we read from left to right,we prefer the context on the left,where it can more effectively familiarize the reader.We prefer the new,important information on the right,since its job is to intrigue the reader. Information is interpreted more easily and more uniformly if it is placed where most readers expect to find it.These needs and expectations of readers affect the interpretation not only of tables and illustrations but also of prose itself.Readers have relatively fixed expectations about where in the structure of prose they will encounter particular items of its substance.If writers can become consciously aware of these locations,they can better control the degrees of recognition and emphasis a reader will give to the various pieces of information being presented.Good writers are intuitively aware of these expectations;that is why their prose has what we call "shape." This underlying concept of reader expectation is perhaps most immediately evident at the level of the largest units of discourse.(A unit of discourse is defined as anything with a beginning and an end:a clause,a sentence,a section,an article,etc.)A research article,for example,is generally divided into recognizable sections,sometimes labeled Introduction,Experimental Methods,Results and Discussion. When the sections are confused--when too much experimental detail is found in the Results section,or when discussion and results intermingle--readers are often equally confused.In smaller units of discourse the functional divisions are not so explicitly labeled,but readers have definite expectations all
table. Let us say that in tracking the temperature of a liquid over a period of time, an investigator takes measurements every three minutes and records a list of temperatures. Those data could be presented by a number of written structures. Here are two possibilities: t(time)=15’, T(temperature)=32º, t=0’, T=25º; t=6’, T=29º; t=3’, T=27º; t=12’, T=32º; t=9’; T=31º time (min) temperature(ºC) 0 25 3 27 6 29 9 31 12 32 15 32 Precisely the same information appears in both formats, yet most readers find the second easier to interpret. It may be that the very familiarity of the tabular structure makes it easier to use. But, more significantly, the structure of the second table provides the reader with an easily perceived context (time) in which the significant piece of information (temperature) can be interpreted. The contextual material appears on the left in a pattern that produces an expectation of regularity; the interesting results appear on the right in a less obvious pattern, the discovery of which is the point of the table. If the two sides of this simple table are reversed, it becomes much harder to read. temperature(ºC) time(min) 25 0 27 3 29 6 31 9 32 12 32 15 Since we read from left to right, we prefer the context on the left, where it can more effectively familiarize the reader. We prefer the new, important information on the right, since its job is to intrigue the reader. Information is interpreted more easily and more uniformly if it is placed where most readers expect to find it. These needs and expectations of readers affect the interpretation not only of tables and illustrations but also of prose itself. Readers have relatively fixed expectations about where in the structure of prose they will encounter particular items of its substance. If writers can become consciously aware of these locations, they can better control the degrees of recognition and emphasis a reader will give to the various pieces of information being presented. Good writers are intuitively aware of these expectations; that is why their prose has what we call "shape." This underlying concept of reader expectation is perhaps most immediately evident at the level of the largest units of discourse. (A unit of discourse is defined as anything with a beginning and an end: a clause, a sentence, a section, an article, etc.) A research article, for example, is generally divided into recognizable sections, sometimes labeled Introduction, Experimental Methods, Results and Discussion. When the sections are confused--when too much experimental detail is found in the Results section, or when discussion and results intermingle--readers are often equally confused. In smaller units of discourse the functional divisions are not so explicitly labeled, but readers have definite expectations all
the same,and they search for certain information in particular places.If these structural expectations are continually violated,readers are forced to divert energy from understanding the content of a passage to unraveling its structure.As the complexity of the context increases moderately,the possibility of misinterpretation or noninterpretation increases dramatically. We present here some results of applying this methodology to research reports in the scientific literature. We have taken several passages from research articles (either published or accepted for publication)and have suggested ways of rewriting them by applying principles derived from the study of reader expectations.We have not sought to transform the passages into "plain English"for the use of the general public;we have neither decreased the jargon nor diluted the science.We have striven not for simplification but for clarification. Reader Expectations for the Structure of Prose Here is our first example of scientific prose,in its original form: The smallest of the URF's(URFA6L),a 207-nucleotide (nt)reading frame overlapping out of phase the NH,-terminal portion of the adenosinetriphosphatase(ATPase)subunit 6 gene has been identified as the animal equivalent of the recently discovered yeast H-ATPase subunit 8 gene. The functional significance of the other URF's has been,on the contrary,elusive.Recently, however,immunoprecipitation experiments with antibodies to purified,rotenone-sensitive NADH-ubiquinone oxido-reductase [hereafter referred to as respiratory chain NADH dehydrogenase or complex I]from bovine heart,as well as enzyme fractionation studies,have indicated that six human URF's(that is,URF1,URF2,URF3,URF4,URF4L,and URF5, hereafter referred to as ND1,ND2,ND3,ND4,ND4L,and ND5)encode subunits of complex I. This is a large complex that also contains many subunits synthesized in the cytoplasm. [*The full paragraph includes one more sentence:"Support for such functional identification of the URF products has come from the finding that the purified rotenone-sensitive NADH dehydrogenase from Neurospora crassa contains several subunits synthesized within the mitochondria,and from the observation that the stopper mutant of Neurospora crassa,whose mtDNA lacks two genes homologous to URF2 and URF3,has no functional complex I."We have omitted this sentence both because the passage is long enough as is and because it raises no additional structural issues.] Ask any ten people why this paragraph is hard to read,and nine are sure to mention the technical vocabulary;several will also suggest that it requires specialized background knowledge.Those problems turn out to be only a small part of the difficulty.Here is the passage again,with the difficult words temporarily lifted: The smallest of the URF's,and [A],has been identified as a [B]subunit 8 gene.The functional significance of the other URF's has been,on the contrary,elusive.Recently,however,[C] experiments,as well as [D]studies,have indicated that six human URF's [1-6]encode subunits of Complex I.This is a large complex that also contains many subunits synthesized in the cytoplasm. It may now be easier to survive the journey through the prose,but the passage is still difficult.Any number of questions present themselves:What has the first sentence of the passage to do with the last sentence?Does the third sentence contradict what we have been told in the second sentence?Is the functional significance of URF's still "elusive"?Will this passage lead us to further discussion about
the same, and they search for certain information in particular places. If these structural expectations are continually violated, readers are forced to divert energy from understanding the content of a passage to unraveling its structure. As the complexity of the context increases moderately, the possibility of misinterpretation or noninterpretation increases dramatically. We present here some results of applying this methodology to research reports in the scientific literature. We have taken several passages from research articles (either published or accepted for publication) and have suggested ways of rewriting them by applying principles derived from the study of reader expectations. We have not sought to transform the passages into "plain English" for the use of the general public; we have neither decreased the jargon nor diluted the science. We have striven not for simplification but for clarification. Reader Expectations for the Structure of Prose Here is our first example of scientific prose, in its original form: The smallest of the URF’s (URFA6L), a 207-nucleotide (nt) reading frame overlapping out of phase the NH2 -terminal portion of the adenosinetriphosphatase (ATPase) subunit 6 gene has been identified as the animal equivalent of the recently discovered yeast H+ -ATPase subunit 8 gene. The functional significance of the other URF’s has been, on the contrary, elusive. Recently, however, immunoprecipitation experiments with antibodies to purified, rotenone-sensitive NADH-ubiquinone oxido-reductase [hereafter referred to as respiratory chain NADH dehydrogenase or complex I] from bovine heart, as well as enzyme fractionation studies, have indicated that six human URF’s (that is, URF1, URF2, URF3, URF4, URF4L, and URF5, hereafter referred to as ND1, ND2, ND3, ND4, ND4L, and ND5) encode subunits of complex I. This is a large complex that also contains many subunits synthesized in the cytoplasm.* [*The full paragraph includes one more sentence: "Support for such functional identification of the URF products has come from the finding that the purified rotenone-sensitive NADH dehydrogenase from Neurospora crassa contains several subunits synthesized within the mitochondria, and from the observation that the stopper mutant of Neurospora crassa, whose mtDNA lacks two genes homologous to URF2 and URF3, has no functional complex I." We have omitted this sentence both because the passage is long enough as is and because it raises no additional structural issues.] Ask any ten people why this paragraph is hard to read, and nine are sure to mention the technical vocabulary; several will also suggest that it requires specialized background knowledge. Those problems turn out to be only a small part of the difficulty. Here is the passage again, with the difficult words temporarily lifted: The smallest of the URF’s, and [A], has been identified as a [B] subunit 8 gene. The functional significance of the other URF’s has been, on the contrary, elusive. Recently, however, [C] experiments, as well as [D] studies, have indicated that six human URF’s [1-6] encode subunits of Complex I. This is a large complex that also contains many subunits synthesized in the cytoplasm. It may now be easier to survive the journey through the prose, but the passage is still difficult. Any number of questions present themselves: What has the first sentence of the passage to do with the last sentence? Does the third sentence contradict what we have been told in the second sentence? Is the functional significance of URF’s still "elusive"? Will this passage lead us to further discussion about
URF's,or about Complex I,or both? Information is interpreted more easily and more uniformly if it is placed where most readers expect to find it. Knowing a little about the subject matter does not clear up all the confusion.The intended audience of this passage would probably possess at least two items of essential technical information:first,"URF" stands for "Uninterrupted Reading Frame,which describes a segment of DNA organized in such a way that it could encode a protein,although no such protein product has yet been identified;second,both APTase and NADH oxido-reductase are enzyme complexes central to energy metabolism.Although this information may provide some sense of comfort,it does little to answer the interpretive questions that need answering.It seems the reader is hindered by more than just the scientific jargon. To get at the problem,we need to articulate something about how readers go about reading.We proceed to the first of several reader expectations. Subject-Verb Separation Look again at the first sentence of the passage cited above.It is relatively long,42 words;but that turns out not to be the main cause of its burdensome complexity.Long sentences need not be difficult to read; they are only difficult to write.We have seen sentences of over 100 words that flow easily and persuasively toward their clearly demarcated destination.Those well-wrought serpents all had something in common:Their structure presented information to readers in the order the readers needed and expected it. Beginning with the exciting material and ending with a lack of luster often leaves us disappointed and destroys our sense of momentum. The first sentence of our example passage does just the opposite:it burdens and obstructs the reader, because of an all-too-common structural defect.Note that the grammatical subject("the smallest")is separated from its verb("has been identified")by 23 words,more than half the sentence.Readers expect a grammatical subject to be followed immediately by the verb.Anything of length that intervenes between subject and verb is read as an interruption,and therefore as something of lesser importance
URF’s, or about Complex I, or both? Information is interpreted more easily and more uniformly if it is placed where most readers expect to find it. Knowing a little about the subject matter does not clear up all the confusion. The intended audience of this passage would probably possess at least two items of essential technical information: first, "URF" stands for "Uninterrupted Reading Frame," which describes a segment of DNA organized in such a way that it could encode a protein, although no such protein product has yet been identified; second, both APTase and NADH oxido-reductase are enzyme complexes central to energy metabolism. Although this information may provide some sense of comfort, it does little to answer the interpretive questions that need answering. It seems the reader is hindered by more than just the scientific jargon. To get at the problem, we need to articulate something about how readers go about reading. We proceed to the first of several reader expectations. Subject-Verb Separation Look again at the first sentence of the passage cited above. It is relatively long, 42 words; but that turns out not to be the main cause of its burdensome complexity. Long sentences need not be difficult to read; they are only difficult to write. We have seen sentences of over 100 words that flow easily and persuasively toward their clearly demarcated destination. Those well-wrought serpents all had something in common: Their structure presented information to readers in the order the readers needed and expected it. Beginning with the exciting material and ending with a lack of luster often leaves us disappointed and destroys our sense of momentum. The first sentence of our example passage does just the opposite: it burdens and obstructs the reader, because of an all-too-common structural defect. Note that the grammatical subject ("the smallest") is separated from its verb ("has been identified") by 23 words, more than half the sentence. Readers expect a grammatical subject to be followed immediately by the verb. Anything of length that intervenes between subject and verb is read as an interruption, and therefore as something of lesser importance
The reader's expectation stems from a pressing need for syntactic resolution,fulfilled only by the arrival of the verb.Without the verb,we do not know what the subject is doing,or what the sentence is all about.As a result,the reader focuses attention on the arrival of the verb and resists recognizing anything in the interrupting material as being of primary importance.The longer the interruption lasts,the more likely it becomes that the "interruptive"material actually contains important information;but its structural location will continue to brand it as merely interruptive.Unfortunately,the reader will not discover its true value until too late-until the sentence has ended without having produced anything of much value outside of that subject-verb interruption. In this first sentence of the paragraph,the relative importance of the intervening material is difficult to evaluate.The material might conceivably be quite significant,in which case the writer should have positioned it to reveal that importance.Here is one way to incorporate it into the sentence structure: The smallest of the URF's is URFA6L,a 207-nucleotide(nt)reading frame overlapping out of phase the NH2-terminal portion of the adenosinetriphosphatase(ATPase)subunit 6 gene;it has been identified as the animal equivalent of the recently discovered yeast H-ATPase subunit 8 gene. On the other hand,the intervening material might be a mere aside that diverts attention from more important ideas;in that case the writer should have deleted it,allowing the prose to drive more directly toward its significant point: The smallest of the URF's(URFA6L)has been identified as the animal equivalent of the recently discovered yeast H-ATPase subunit 8 gene. Only the author could tell us which of these revisions more accurately reflects his intentions. These revisions lead us to a second set of reader expectations.Each unit of discourse,no matter what the size,is expected to serve a single function,to make a single point.In the case of a sentence,the point is expected to appear in a specific place reserved for emphasis. The Stress Position It is a linguistic commonplace that readers naturally emphasize the material that arrives at the end of a sentence.We refer to that location as a "stress position."If a writer is consciously aware of this tendency,she can arrange for the emphatic information to appear at the moment the reader is naturally exerting the greatest reading emphasis.As a result,the chances greatly increase that reader and writer will perceive the same material as being worthy of primary emphasis.The very structure of the sentence thus helps persuade the reader of the relative values of the sentence's contents. The inclination to direct more energy to that which arrives last in a sentence seems to correspond to the way we work at tasks through time.We tend to take something like a "mental breath"as we begin to read each new sentence,thereby summoning the tension with which we pay attention to the unfolding of the syntax.As we recognize that the sentence is drawing toward its conclusion,we begin to exhale that mental breath.The exhalation produces a sense of emphasis.Moreover,we delight in being rewarded at the end of a labor with something that makes the ongoing effort worthwhile.Beginning with the exciting
The reader’s expectation stems from a pressing need for syntactic resolution, fulfilled only by the arrival of the verb. Without the verb, we do not know what the subject is doing, or what the sentence is all about. As a result, the reader focuses attention on the arrival of the verb and resists recognizing anything in the interrupting material as being of primary importance. The longer the interruption lasts, the more likely it becomes that the "interruptive" material actually contains important information; but its structural location will continue to brand it as merely interruptive. Unfortunately, the reader will not discover its true value until too late-until the sentence has ended without having produced anything of much value outside of that subject-verb interruption. In this first sentence of the paragraph, the relative importance of the intervening material is difficult to evaluate. The material might conceivably be quite significant, in which case the writer should have positioned it to reveal that importance. Here is one way to incorporate it into the sentence structure: The smallest of the URF’s is URFA6L, a 207-nucleotide (nt) reading frame overlapping out of phase the NH2 -terminal portion of the adenosinetriphosphatase (ATPase) subunit 6 gene; it has been identified as the animal equivalent of the recently discovered yeast H+ -ATPase subunit 8 gene. On the other hand, the intervening material might be a mere aside that diverts attention from more important ideas; in that case the writer should have deleted it, allowing the prose to drive more directly toward its significant point: The smallest of the URF’s (URFA6L) has been identified as the animal equivalent of the recently discovered yeast H+ -ATPase subunit 8 gene. Only the author could tell us which of these revisions more accurately reflects his intentions. These revisions lead us to a second set of reader expectations. Each unit of discourse, no matter what the size, is expected to serve a single function, to make a single point. In the case of a sentence, the point is expected to appear in a specific place reserved for emphasis. The Stress Position It is a linguistic commonplace that readers naturally emphasize the material that arrives at the end of a sentence. We refer to that location as a "stress position." If a writer is consciously aware of this tendency, she can arrange for the emphatic information to appear at the moment the reader is naturally exerting the greatest reading emphasis. As a result, the chances greatly increase that reader and writer will perceive the same material as being worthy of primary emphasis. The very structure of the sentence thus helps persuade the reader of the relative values of the sentence’s contents. The inclination to direct more energy to that which arrives last in a sentence seems to correspond to the way we work at tasks through time. We tend to take something like a "mental breath" as we begin to read each new sentence, thereby summoning the tension with which we pay attention to the unfolding of the syntax. As we recognize that the sentence is drawing toward its conclusion, we begin to exhale that mental breath. The exhalation produces a sense of emphasis. Moreover, we delight in being rewarded at the end of a labor with something that makes the ongoing effort worthwhile. Beginning with the exciting
material and ending with a lack of luster often leaves us disappointed and destroys our sense of momentum.We do not start with the strawberry shortcake and work our way up to the broccoli. When the writer puts the emphatic material of a sentence in any place other than the stress position,one of two things can happen;both are bad.First,the reader might find the stress position occupied by material that clearly is not worthy of emphasis.In this case,the reader must discern,without any additional structural clue,what else in the sentence may be the most likely candidate for emphasis There are no secondary structural indications to fall back upon.In sentences that are long,dense or sophisticated,chances soar that the reader will not interpret the prose precisely as the writer intended. The second possibility is even worse:The reader may find the stress position occupied by something that does appear capable of receiving emphasis,even though the writer did not intend to give it any stress.In that case,the reader is highly likely to emphasize this imposter material,and the writer will have lost an important opportunity to influence the reader's interpretive process. The stress position can change in size from sentence to sentence.Sometimes it consists of a single word; sometimes it extends to several lines.The definitive factor is this:The stress position coincides with the moment of syntactic closure.A reader has reached the beginning of the stress position when she knows there is nothing left in the clause or sentence but the material presently being read.Thus a whole list, numbered and indented,can occupy the stress position of a sentence if it has been clearly announced as being all that remains of that sentence.Each member of that list,in turn,may have its own internal stress position,since each member may produce its own syntactic closure. Within a sentence,secondary stress positions can be formed by the appearance of a properly used colon or semicolon;by grammatical convention,the material preceding these punctuation marks must be able to stand by itself as a complete sentence.Thus,sentences can be extended effortlessly to dozens of words,as long as there is a medial syntactic closure for every piece of new,stress-worthy information along the way.One of our revisions of the initial sentence can serve as an example: The smallest of the URF's is URFA6L,a 207-nucleotide(nt)reading frame overlapping out of phase the NH-terminal portion of the adenosinetriphosphatase(ATPase)subunit 6 gene;it has been identified as the animal equivalent of the recently discovered yeast H-ATPase subunit 8 gene. By using a semicolon,we created a second stress position to accommodate a second piece of information that seemed to require emphasis. We now have three rhetorical principles based on reader expectations:First,grammatical subjects should be followed as soon as possible by their verbs;second,every unit of discourse,no matter the size,should serve a single function or make a single point;and,third,information intended to be emphasized should appear at points of syntactic closure.Using these principles,we can begin to unravel the problems of our example prose. Note the subject-verb separation in the 62-word third sentence of the original passage: Recently,however,immunoprecipitation experiments with antibodies to purified, rotenone-sensitive NADH-ubiquinone oxido-reductase [hereafter referred to as respiratory chain NADH dehydrogenase or complex I]from bovine heart,as well as enzyme fractionation studies, have indicated that six human URF's(that is,URF1,URF2,URF3,URF4,URF4L,and URF5
material and ending with a lack of luster often leaves us disappointed and destroys our sense of momentum. We do not start with the strawberry shortcake and work our way up to the broccoli. When the writer puts the emphatic material of a sentence in any place other than the stress position, one of two things can happen; both are bad. First, the reader might find the stress position occupied by material that clearly is not worthy of emphasis. In this case, the reader must discern, without any additional structural clue, what else in the sentence may be the most likely candidate for emphasis. There are no secondary structural indications to fall back upon. In sentences that are long, dense or sophisticated, chances soar that the reader will not interpret the prose precisely as the writer intended. The second possibility is even worse: The reader may find the stress position occupied by something that does appear capable of receiving emphasis, even though the writer did not intend to give it any stress. In that case, the reader is highly likely to emphasize this imposter material, and the writer will have lost an important opportunity to influence the reader’s interpretive process. The stress position can change in size from sentence to sentence. Sometimes it consists of a single word; sometimes it extends to several lines. The definitive factor is this: The stress position coincides with the moment of syntactic closure. A reader has reached the beginning of the stress position when she knows there is nothing left in the clause or sentence but the material presently being read. Thus a whole list, numbered and indented, can occupy the stress position of a sentence if it has been clearly announced as being all that remains of that sentence. Each member of that list, in turn, may have its own internal stress position, since each member may produce its own syntactic closure. Within a sentence, secondary stress positions can be formed by the appearance of a properly used colon or semicolon; by grammatical convention, the material preceding these punctuation marks must be able to stand by itself as a complete sentence. Thus, sentences can be extended effortlessly to dozens of words, as long as there is a medial syntactic closure for every piece of new, stress-worthy information along the way. One of our revisions of the initial sentence can serve as an example: The smallest of the URF’s is URFA6L, a 207-nucleotide (nt) reading frame overlapping out of phase the NH2 -terminal portion of the adenosinetriphosphatase (ATPase) subunit 6 gene; it has been identified as the animal equivalent of the recently discovered yeast H+ -ATPase subunit 8 gene. By using a semicolon, we created a second stress position to accommodate a second piece of information that seemed to require emphasis. We now have three rhetorical principles based on reader expectations: First, grammatical subjects should be followed as soon as possible by their verbs; second, every unit of discourse, no matter the size, should serve a single function or make a single point; and, third, information intended to be emphasized should appear at points of syntactic closure. Using these principles, we can begin to unravel the problems of our example prose. Note the subject-verb separation in the 62-word third sentence of the original passage: Recently, however, immunoprecipitation experiments with antibodies to purified, rotenone-sensitive NADH-ubiquinone oxido-reductase [hereafter referred to as respiratory chain NADH dehydrogenase or complex I] from bovine heart, as well as enzyme fractionation studies, have indicated that six human URF’s (that is, URF1, URF2, URF3, URF4, URF4L, and URF5
hereafter referred to as ND1,ND2,ND3,ND4,ND4L and ND5)encode subunits of complex I. After encountering the subject("experiments"),the reader must wade through 27 words(including three hyphenated compound words,a parenthetical interruption and an"as well as"phrase)before alighting on the highly uninformative and disappointingly anticlimactic verb("have indicated").Without a moment to recover,the reader is handed a "that"clause in which the new subject("six human URF's")is separated from its verb("encode")by yet another 20 words. If we applied the three principles we have developed to the rest of the sentences of the example,we could generate a great many revised versions of each.These revisions might differ significantly from one another in the way their structures indicate to the reader the various weights and balances to be given to the information.Had the author placed all stress-worthy material in stress positions,we as a reading community would have been far more likely to interpret these sentences uniformly. We couch this discussion in terms of "likelihood"because we believe that meaning is not inherent in discourse by itself;"meaning"requires the combined participation of text and reader.All sentences are infinitely interpretable,given an infinite number of interpreters.As communities of readers,however, we tend to work out tacit agreements as to what kinds of meaning are most likely to be extracted from certain articulations.We cannot succeed in making even a single sentence mean one and only one thing; we can only increase the odds that a large majority of readers will tend to interpret our discourse according to our intentions.Such success will follow from authors becoming more consciously aware of the various reader expectations presented here. We cannot succeed in making even a single sentence mean one and only one thing;we can only increase the odds that a large majority ofreaders will tend to interpret our discourse according to our intentions. Here is one set of revisionary decisions we made for the example: The smallest of the URF's,URFA6L,has been identified as the animal equivalent of the recently discovered yeast H-ATPase subunit 8 gene;but the functional significance of other URF's has been more elusive.Recently,however,several human URF's have been shown to encode subunits of rotenone-sensitive NADH-ubiquinone oxido-reductase.This is a large complex that also contains many subunits synthesized in the cytoplasm;it will be referred to hereafter as respiratory chain NADH dehydrogenase or complex I.Six subunits of Complex I were shown by enzyme fractionation studies and immunoprecipitation experiments to be encoded by six human URF's (URF1,URF2,URF3,URF4,URF4L,and URF5);these URF's will be referred to subsequently as ND1,ND2,ND3,ND4,ND4L and ND5
hereafter referred to as ND1, ND2, ND3, ND4, ND4L and ND5) encode subunits of complex I. After encountering the subject ("experiments"), the reader must wade through 27 words (including three hyphenated compound words, a parenthetical interruption and an "as well as" phrase) before alighting on the highly uninformative and disappointingly anticlimactic verb ("have indicated"). Without a moment to recover, the reader is handed a "that" clause in which the new subject ("six human URF’s") is separated from its verb ("encode") by yet another 20 words. If we applied the three principles we have developed to the rest of the sentences of the example, we could generate a great many revised versions of each. These revisions might differ significantly from one another in the way their structures indicate to the reader the various weights and balances to be given to the information. Had the author placed all stress-worthy material in stress positions, we as a reading community would have been far more likely to interpret these sentences uniformly. We couch this discussion in terms of "likelihood" because we believe that meaning is not inherent in discourse by itself; "meaning" requires the combined participation of text and reader. All sentences are infinitely interpretable, given an infinite number of interpreters. As communities of readers, however, we tend to work out tacit agreements as to what kinds of meaning are most likely to be extracted from certain articulations. We cannot succeed in making even a single sentence mean one and only one thing; we can only increase the odds that a large majority of readers will tend to interpret our discourse according to our intentions. Such success will follow from authors becoming more consciously aware of the various reader expectations presented here. We cannot succeed in making even a single sentence mean one and only one thing; we can only increase the odds that a large majority of readers will tend to interpret our discourse according to our intentions. Here is one set of revisionary decisions we made for the example: The smallest of the URF’s, URFA6L, has been identified as the animal equivalent of the recently discovered yeast H+ -ATPase subunit 8 gene; but the functional significance of other URF’s has been more elusive. Recently, however, several human URF’s have been shown to encode subunits of rotenone-sensitive NADH-ubiquinone oxido-reductase. This is a large complex that also contains many subunits synthesized in the cytoplasm; it will be referred to hereafter as respiratory chain NADH dehydrogenase or complex I. Six subunits of Complex I were shown by enzyme fractionation studies and immunoprecipitation experiments to be encoded by six human URF’s (URF1, URF2, URF3, URF4, URF4L, and URF5); these URF’s will be referred to subsequently as ND1, ND2, ND3, ND4, ND4L and ND5
Sheer length was neither the problem nor the solution.The revised version is not noticeably shorter than the original;nevertheless,it is significantly easier to interpret.We have indeed deleted certain words, but not on the basis of wordiness or excess length.(See especially the last sentence of our revision.) When is a sentence too long?The creators of readability formulas would have us believe there exists some fixed number of words (the favorite is 29)past which a sentence is too hard to read.We disagree. We have seen 10-word sentences that are virtually impenetrable and,as we mentioned above,100-word sentences that flow effortlessly to their points of resolution.In place of the word-limit concept,we offer the following definition:A sentence is too long when it has more viable candidates for stress positions than there are stress positions available.Without the stress position's locational clue that its material is intended to be emphasized,readers are left too much to their own devices in deciding just what else in a sentence might be considered important. In revising the example passage,we made certain decisions about what to omit and what to emphasize. We put subjects and verbs together to lessen the reader's syntactic burdens;we put the material we believed worthy of emphasis in stress positions;and we discarded material for which we could not discern significant connections.In doing so,we have produced a clearer passage--but not one that necessarily reflects the author's intentions;it reflects only our interpretation of the author's intentions. The more problematic the structure,the less likely it becomes that a grand majority of readers will perceive the discourse in exactly the way the author intended. The information that begins a sentence establishes for the reader a perspective for viewing the sentence as a unit. It is probable that many of our readers--and perhaps even the authors--will disagree with some of our choices.If so,that disagreement underscores our point:The original failed to communicate its ideas and their connections clearly.If we happened to have interpreted the passage as you did,then we can make a different point:No one should have to work as hard as we did to unearth the content of a single passage of this length. The Topic Position To summarize the principles connected with the stress position,we have the proverbial wisdom,"Save the best for last."To summarize the principles connected with the other end of the sentence,which we will call the topic position,we have its proverbial contradiction,"First things first."In the stress position the reader needs and expects closure and fulfillment;in the topic position the reader needs and expects perspective and context.With so much of reading comprehension affected by what shows up in the topic position,it behooves a writer to control what appears at the beginning of sentences with great care. The information that begins a sentence establishes for the reader a perspective for viewing the sentence
Sheer length was neither the problem nor the solution. The revised version is not noticeably shorter than the original; nevertheless, it is significantly easier to interpret. We have indeed deleted certain words, but not on the basis of wordiness or excess length. (See especially the last sentence of our revision.) When is a sentence too long? The creators of readability formulas would have us believe there exists some fixed number of words (the favorite is 29) past which a sentence is too hard to read. We disagree. We have seen 10-word sentences that are virtually impenetrable and, as we mentioned above, 100-word sentences that flow effortlessly to their points of resolution. In place of the word-limit concept, we offer the following definition: A sentence is too long when it has more viable candidates for stress positions than there are stress positions available. Without the stress position’s locational clue that its material is intended to be emphasized, readers are left too much to their own devices in deciding just what else in a sentence might be considered important. In revising the example passage, we made certain decisions about what to omit and what to emphasize. We put subjects and verbs together to lessen the reader’s syntactic burdens; we put the material we believed worthy of emphasis in stress positions; and we discarded material for which we could not discern significant connections. In doing so, we have produced a clearer passage--but not one that necessarily reflects the author’s intentions; it reflects only our interpretation of the author’s intentions. The more problematic the structure, the less likely it becomes that a grand majority of readers will perceive the discourse in exactly the way the author intended. The information that begins a sentence establishes for the reader a perspective for viewing the sentence as a unit. It is probable that many of our readers--and perhaps even the authors--will disagree with some of our choices. If so, that disagreement underscores our point: The original failed to communicate its ideas and their connections clearly. If we happened to have interpreted the passage as you did, then we can make a different point: No one should have to work as hard as we did to unearth the content of a single passage of this length. The Topic Position To summarize the principles connected with the stress position, we have the proverbial wisdom, "Save the best for last." To summarize the principles connected with the other end of the sentence, which we will call the topic position, we have its proverbial contradiction, "First things first." In the stress position the reader needs and expects closure and fulfillment; in the topic position the reader needs and expects perspective and context. With so much of reading comprehension affected by what shows up in the topic position, it behooves a writer to control what appears at the beginning of sentences with great care. The information that begins a sentence establishes for the reader a perspective for viewing the sentence
as a unit:Readers expect a unit of discourse to be a story about whoever shows up first."Bees disperse pollen"and "Pollen is dispersed by bees"are two different but equally respectable sentences about the same facts.The first tells us something about bees;the second tells us something about pollen.The passivity of the second sentence does not by itself impair its quality;in fact,"Pollen is dispersed by bees"is the superior sentence if it appears in a paragraph that intends to tell us a continuing story about pollen.Pollen's story at that moment is a passive one. Readers also expect the material occupying the topic position to provide them with linkage (looking backward)and context (looking forward).The information in the topic position prepares the reader for upcoming material by connecting it backward to the previous discussion.Although linkage and context can derive from several sources,they stem primarily from material that the reader has already encountered within this particular piece of discourse.We refer to this familiar,previously introduced material as"old information."Conversely,material making its first appearance in a discourse is"new information."When new information is important enough to receive emphasis,it functions best in the stress position. When old information consistently arrives in the topic position,it helps readers to construct the logical flow of the argument:It focuses attention on one particular strand of the discussion,both harkening backward and leaning forward.In contrast,if the topic position is constantly occupied by material that fails to establish linkage and context,readers will have difficulty perceiving both the connection to the previous sentence and the projected role of the new sentence in the development of the paragraph as a whole Here is a second example of scientific prose that we shall attempt to improve in subsequent discussion: Large earthquakes along a given fault segment do not occur at random intervals because it takes time to accumulate the strain energy for the rupture.The rates at which tectonic plates move and accumulate strain at their boundaries are approximately uniform.Therefore,in first approximation, one may expect that large ruptures of the same fault segment will occur at approximately constant time intervals.If subsequent main shocks have different amounts of slip across the fault,then the recurrence time may vary,and the basic idea of periodic mainshocks must be modified.For great plate boundary ruptures the length and slip often vary by a factor of 2.Along the southern segment of the San Andreas fault the recurrence interval is 145 years with variations of several decades. The smaller the standard deviation of the average recurrence interval,the more specific could be the long term prediction of a future mainshock. This is the kind of passage that in subtle ways can make readers feel badly about themselves.The individual sentences give the impression of being intelligently fashioned:They are not especially long or convoluted;their vocabulary is appropriately professional but not beyond the ken of educated general readers;and they are free of grammatical and dictional errors.On first reading,however,many of us arrive at the paragraph's end without a clear sense of where we have been or where we are going.When that happens,we tend to berate ourselves for not having paid close enough attention.In reality,the fault lies not with us,but with the author. We can distill the problem by looking closely at the information in each sentence's topic position: Large earthquakes The rates
as a unit: Readers expect a unit of discourse to be a story about whoever shows up first. "Bees disperse pollen" and "Pollen is dispersed by bees" are two different but equally respectable sentences about the same facts. The first tells us something about bees; the second tells us something about pollen. The passivity of the second sentence does not by itself impair its quality; in fact, "Pollen is dispersed by bees" is the superior sentence if it appears in a paragraph that intends to tell us a continuing story about pollen. Pollen’s story at that moment is a passive one. Readers also expect the material occupying the topic position to provide them with linkage (looking backward) and context (looking forward). The information in the topic position prepares the reader for upcoming material by connecting it backward to the previous discussion. Although linkage and context can derive from several sources, they stem primarily from material that the reader has already encountered within this particular piece of discourse. We refer to this familiar, previously introduced material as "old information." Conversely, material making its first appearance in a discourse is "new information." When new information is important enough to receive emphasis, it functions best in the stress position. When old information consistently arrives in the topic position, it helps readers to construct the logical flow of the argument: It focuses attention on one particular strand of the discussion, both harkening backward and leaning forward. In contrast, if the topic position is constantly occupied by material that fails to establish linkage and context, readers will have difficulty perceiving both the connection to the previous sentence and the projected role of the new sentence in the development of the paragraph as a whole. Here is a second example of scientific prose that we shall attempt to improve in subsequent discussion: Large earthquakes along a given fault segment do not occur at random intervals because it takes time to accumulate the strain energy for the rupture. The rates at which tectonic plates move and accumulate strain at their boundaries are approximately uniform. Therefore, in first approximation, one may expect that large ruptures of the same fault segment will occur at approximately constant time intervals. If subsequent main shocks have different amounts of slip across the fault, then the recurrence time may vary, and the basic idea of periodic mainshocks must be modified. For great plate boundary ruptures the length and slip often vary by a factor of 2. Along the southern segment of the San Andreas fault the recurrence interval is 145 years with variations of several decades. The smaller the standard deviation of the average recurrence interval, the more specific could be the long term prediction of a future mainshock. This is the kind of passage that in subtle ways can make readers feel badly about themselves. The individual sentences give the impression of being intelligently fashioned: They are not especially long or convoluted; their vocabulary is appropriately professional but not beyond the ken of educated general readers; and they are free of grammatical and dictional errors. On first reading, however, many of us arrive at the paragraph’s end without a clear sense of where we have been or where we are going. When that happens, we tend to berate ourselves for not having paid close enough attention. In reality, the fault lies not with us, but with the author. We can distill the problem by looking closely at the information in each sentence’s topic position: Large earthquakes The rates
Therefore...one subsequent mainshocks great plate boundary ruptures the southern segment of the San Andreas fault the smaller the standard deviation... Much of this information is making its first appearance in this paragraph--in precisely the spot where the reader looks for old,familiar information.As a result,the focus of the story constantly shifts.Given just the material in the topic positions,no two readers would be likely to construct exactly the same story for the paragraph as a whole. If we try to piece together the relationship of each sentence to its neighbors,we notice that certain bits of old information keep reappearing.We hear a good deal about the recurrence time between earthquakes: The first sentence introduces the concept of nonrandom intervals between earthquakes;the second sentence tells us that recurrence rates due to the movement of tectonic plates are more or less uniform; the third sentence adds that the recurrence rates of major earthquakes should also be somewhat predictable;the fourth sentence adds that recurrence rates vary with some conditions;the fifth sentence adds information about one particular variation;the sixth sentence adds a recurrence-rate example from California;and the last sentence tells us something about how recurrence rates can be described statistically.This refrain of "recurrence intervals"constitutes the major string of old information in the paragraph.Unfortunately,it rarely appears at the beginning of sentences,where it would help us maintain our focus on its continuing story. In reading,as in most experiences,we appreciate the opportunity to become familiar with a new environment before having to function in it.Writing that continually begins sentences with new information and ends with old information forbids both the sense of comfort and orientation at the start and the sense of fulfilling arrival at the end.It misleads the reader as to whose story is being told;it burdens the reader with new information that must be carried further into the sentence before it can be connected to the discussion;and it creates ambiguity as to which material the writer intended the reader to emphasize.All of these distractions require that readers expend a disproportionate amount of energy to unravel the structure of the prose,leaving less energy available for perceiving content. We can begin to revise the example by ensuring the following for each sentence: 1.The backward-linking old information appears in the topic position. 2.The person,thing or concept whose story it is appears in the topic position. 3.The new,emphasis-worthy information appears in the stress position Once again,if our decisions concerning the relative values of specific information differ from yours,we can all blame the author,who failed to make his intentions apparent.Here first is a list of what we perceived to be the new,emphatic material in each sentence: time to accumulate strain energy along a fault approximately uniform large ruptures of the same fault different amounts of slip vary by a factor of 2 variations of several decades
Therefore...one subsequent mainshocks great plate boundary ruptures the southern segment of the San Andreas fault the smaller the standard deviation... Much of this information is making its first appearance in this paragraph--in precisely the spot where the reader looks for old, familiar information. As a result, the focus of the story constantly shifts. Given just the material in the topic positions, no two readers would be likely to construct exactly the same story for the paragraph as a whole. If we try to piece together the relationship of each sentence to its neighbors, we notice that certain bits of old information keep reappearing. We hear a good deal about the recurrence time between earthquakes: The first sentence introduces the concept of nonrandom intervals between earthquakes; the second sentence tells us that recurrence rates due to the movement of tectonic plates are more or less uniform; the third sentence adds that the recurrence rates of major earthquakes should also be somewhat predictable; the fourth sentence adds that recurrence rates vary with some conditions; the fifth sentence adds information about one particular variation; the sixth sentence adds a recurrence-rate example from California; and the last sentence tells us something about how recurrence rates can be described statistically. This refrain of "recurrence intervals" constitutes the major string of old information in the paragraph. Unfortunately, it rarely appears at the beginning of sentences, where it would help us maintain our focus on its continuing story. In reading, as in most experiences, we appreciate the opportunity to become familiar with a new environment before having to function in it. Writing that continually begins sentences with new information and ends with old information forbids both the sense of comfort and orientation at the start and the sense of fulfilling arrival at the end. It misleads the reader as to whose story is being told; it burdens the reader with new information that must be carried further into the sentence before it can be connected to the discussion; and it creates ambiguity as to which material the writer intended the reader to emphasize. All of these distractions require that readers expend a disproportionate amount of energy to unravel the structure of the prose, leaving less energy available for perceiving content. We can begin to revise the example by ensuring the following for each sentence: 1. The backward-linking old information appears in the topic position. 2. The person, thing or concept whose story it is appears in the topic position. 3. The new, emphasis-worthy information appears in the stress position. Once again, if our decisions concerning the relative values of specific information differ from yours, we can all blame the author, who failed to make his intentions apparent. Here first is a list of what we perceived to be the new, emphatic material in each sentence: time to accumulate strain energy along a fault approximately uniform large ruptures of the same fault different amounts of slip vary by a factor of 2 variations of several decades